NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Publication Date
In 20250
Since 20240
Since 2021 (last 5 years)0
Since 2016 (last 10 years)4
Since 2006 (last 20 years)16
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Wechsler Preschool and…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 135 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
D'Arms, Justin; Samuels, Richard – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Emotion development research centrally concerns capacities to produce emotions and to think about them. We distinguish these enterprises and consider a novel account of how they might be related. On one recent account, the capacity to have emotions of various kinds comes by way of the acquisition of emotion concepts. This account relies on a…
Descriptors: Emotional Development, Concept Formation, Constructivism (Learning), Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mary Beth Patry; Eva Marie Horn – Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2019
The purpose of this article was to synthesize the available research regarding the development of complex schemata in individuals with autism across its entire developmental process beginning with prototype formation, followed by categorization, and finally the development of schema. Specific research questions addressed the quality of research…
Descriptors: Schemata (Cognition), Concept Formation, Autism Spectrum Disorders, Cognitive Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hoemann, Katie; Xu, Fei; Barrett, Lisa Feldman – Developmental Psychology, 2019
In this article, we integrate two constructionist approaches--the theory of constructed emotion and rational constructivism--to introduce several novel hypotheses for understanding emotional development. We first discuss the hypothesis that emotion categories are abstract and conceptual, whose instances share a goal-based function in a particular…
Descriptors: Emotional Development, Child Development, Psychological Patterns, Vocabulary
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tzuriel, David; Isman, Esther B.; Klung, Tamar; Haywood, H. Carl – Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 2017
Children, 4-6 years of age, in special education kindergartens were randomly assigned to a classification training (n 45) and a comparison (n 49) group. Children in the training group were taught the Classification unit of Bright Start, whereas those in the comparison group received a regular content-oriented curriculum. Both groups were given…
Descriptors: Young Children, Kindergarten, Special Education, Students with Disabilities
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Chalik, Lisa; Rhodes, Marjorie – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2014
Developing mechanisms for predicting human action is a critical task of early conceptual development. Three studies examined whether 4-year-old children (N = 149) use social allegiances to predict behavior, by testing whether they expect the experiences of social partners to influence individual action. After being exposed to a conflict between…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Prediction, Friendship, Conflict
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Prasada, Sandeep; Hennefield, Laura; Otap, Daniel – Cognitive Science, 2012
We investigate the hypothesis that our conceptual systems provide two formally distinct ways of representing categories by investigating the manner in which lexical nominals (e.g., "tree," "picnic table") and phrasal nominals (e.g., "black bird," "birds that like rice") are interpreted. Four experiments found that lexical nominals may be mapped…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Cognitive Development, Classification, Nouns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Fisher, Anna V. – Cognition, 2011
Is processing of conceptual information as robust as processing of perceptual information early in development? Existing empirical evidence is insufficient to answer this question. To examine this issue, 3- to 5-year-old children were presented with a flexible categorization task, in which target items (e.g., an open red umbrella) shared category…
Descriptors: Test Items, Classification, Preschool Children, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kidd, Julie K.; Curby, Timothy W.; Boyer, Caroline E.; Gadzichowski, K. Marinka; Gallington, Deborah A.; Machado, Jessica A.; Pasnak, Robert – Early Education and Development, 2012
Research Findings: A total of 72 Head Start children (M age = 53.26 months, SD = 5.07) were randomly assigned to 4 conditions. Some were taught the oddity principle (choosing the object that differs from others in a group) and seriation (ordering objects on a dimension and inserting new objects into such orders), which are forms of thinking that…
Descriptors: Art Activities, Disadvantaged Youth, Economically Disadvantaged, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Blaye, Agnes; Jacques, Sophie – Developmental Science, 2009
The current study evaluated the relative roles of conceptual knowledge and executive control on the development of "categorical flexibility," the ability to switch between simultaneously available but conflicting categorical representations of an object. Experiment 1 assessed conceptual knowledge and executive control together; Experiment 2…
Descriptors: Concept Formation, Preschool Children, Cognitive Processes, Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Nguyen, Simone P. – Developmental Psychology, 2007
Items commonly belong to many categories. Cross-classification is the classification of a single item into more than one category. This research explored 2- to 6-year-old children's use of 2 different category systems for cross-classification: script (e.g., school-time items, birthday party items) and taxonomic (e.g., animals, clothes). The…
Descriptors: Classification, Young Children, Child Development, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bernstein, Debra; Crowley, Kevin – Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2008
Children's worlds are increasingly populated by intelligent technologies. This has raised a number of questions about the ways in which technology can change children's ideas about important concepts, like what it means to be alive or smart. In this study, we examined the impact of experience with intelligent technologies on children's ideas about…
Descriptors: Intelligence, Individual Characteristics, Concept Formation, Robotics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Nelson, Deborah G. Kemler; O'Neil, Kelly A.; Asher, Yvonne M. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2008
Two studies investigated the relationship between learning names and learning concepts in preschool children. More specifically, we focused on the relationship between learning the names and learning the intended functions of artifacts, given that the intended function of an artifact is generally thought to constitute core conceptual information…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Classification, Correlation, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Sarama, Julie; Clements, Douglas H. – American Journal of Play, 2009
The authors explore how children's play can support the development of the foundations of mathematics learning and how adults can support children's representation of--and thus the "mathematization" of--their play. The authors review research about the amount and nature of mathematics found in the free play of children. They briefly…
Descriptors: Play, Cognitive Development, Child Development, Mathematics Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gelman, Susan A.; Bloom, Paul – Cognition, 2007
Generic sentences (such as "Birds lay eggs") are important in that they refer to kinds (e.g., birds as a group) rather than individuals (e.g., the birds in the henhouse). The present set of studies examined aspects of how generic nouns are understood by English speakers. Adults and children (4- and 5-year-olds) were presented with scenarios about…
Descriptors: Semantics, Sentences, Nouns, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Akman, Berrin; Ipek, Arzu; Uyanik, Gulden – International Journal of Early Years Education, 2000
Used the Bracken Basic Concept Scale to examine whether concepts are related to each other on the conceptual development of 40 six-year-olds. Found significant correlations between the SRC concepts (color, letter, number/counting, comparison, shape) and direction as well as social/emotional concepts and the size concepts. Found that direction…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation, Kindergarten Children
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7  |  8  |  9