Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 4 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Block, Jack | 4 |
Ault, Ruth L. | 3 |
Quay, Lorene C. | 3 |
Adams, Wayne V. | 2 |
Brannigan, Gary G. | 2 |
Brown, Ronald T. | 2 |
Butter, Eliot J. | 2 |
Campbell, Donald S. | 2 |
Dillon, Ronna F. | 2 |
Egeland, Byron | 2 |
Zelniker, Tamar | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 42 |
Journal Articles | 33 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 12 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Information Analyses | 3 |
Opinion Papers | 2 |
Education Level
Elementary Secondary Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 7 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 5 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Cintamulya, Imas – Asia-Pacific Forum on Science Learning and Teaching, 2019
Critical thinking skills, as well as cognitive style, are important aspects that must be considered in assessments of the learning process. This research aims to describe the critical thinking skills of the students with reflective versus impulsive cognitive styles through an explorative study on conservation and environmental knowledge learning.…
Descriptors: Critical Thinking, Thinking Skills, Reflection, Cognitive Style
Aran-Filippetti, Vanessa; Richaud de Minzi, Maria Cristina – Journal of Genetic Psychology, 2012
Socioeconomic status (SES) is a well-known predictor of cognitive achievement and executive functioning, although the underlying cognitive mediating processes remain unclear. The authors analyze the association between different socioeconomic indicators and the executive functions (EF) of schoolchildren and the possible cognitive mediating factors…
Descriptors: Academic Achievement, Intelligence Tests, Intelligence, Intervention
Kenny, Robert – Educational Technology Research and Development, 2009
In recent years, considerable attention has been paid to the alleged changes in the ways the so-called "digital generation" communicates and learns. Most of the commentary has been based on anecdotes, observations and opinions. The lack of empirical evidence in this matter suggests that it may be time to begin the process of gathering…
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Instructional Design, Conceptual Tempo, Influence of Technology
Carretero-Dios, Hugo; Macarena, De los Santos-Roig; Buela-Casal, Gualberto – Learning and Individual Differences, 2008
This study is an item analysis of the Matching Familiar Figures Test-20. We examined error scores in the Matching Familiar Figures Test-20 to determine the influence of the difficulty of the test on the assessment of reflection-impulsivity. The sample included 700 participants aged between 6 and 12 years. The results obtained from the corrected…
Descriptors: Conceptual Tempo, Individual Differences, Item Analysis, Children

Zelniker, Tamar; And Others – Child Development, 1977
Two types of problems similar to those on the Matching Familiar Figures Test were presented to 61 fourth graders. Results indicated that long or short decision time is a consistent trait related to preferred strategy of perceptual analysis, and accuracy depends on compatability between that strategy and specific task requirements. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Style, Conceptual Tempo, Elementary Education

Avila, Cesar; Cuenca, Isabel; Felix, Vicente; Parcet, Maria-Antonia; Miranda, Ana – Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 2004
Seven different laboratory measures of impulsivity were administered to a group of 165 school-aged boys. Parents' and teachers' ratings of Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder and Oppositional/Defiant Disorder were also obtained. Factor analyses of impulsivity measures revealed the existence of a strong Inhibitory Control Factor including…
Descriptors: Performance Tests, Males, Hyperactivity, Conceptual Tempo

Mollick, Lynn R.; Messer, Stanley B. – Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1978
Examines the correlation between reflection-impulsivity scores on the Matching Familiar Figures (MFF) test and scores on the WISC and CTMM intelligence tests. Subjects were 53 ten-year-old girls. Results were discussed in terms of certain characteristics of the tests used and in terms of the age of the subjects. (BD)
Descriptors: Comparative Analysis, Conceptual Tempo, Elementary School Students, Intelligence Tests

Toner, Ignatius J.; And Others – Child Development, 1977
This study explored the relationships among children's performance on a simplified version of Kagan's Matching Familiar Figures (MFF) Test of conceptual tempo, their IQ, their performance on several measures of self-regulatory behavior, and their general activity level. Subjects were 55 preschool boys and girls. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Conceptual Tempo, Intelligence Quotient, Preschool Education, Self Control

Smith, J. David; Caplan, Janet – Developmental Psychology, 1988
A cross-age technique was used in two studies to extend understanding of cognitive style development and its cross-cultural generality. Chinese-American children were given the Matching Familiar Figures Test. Results were compared with existing data to determine the style development of children from other cultures. (PCB)
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Style, Conceptual Tempo, Cross Cultural Studies

Lawry, Janice A.; And Others – Child Development, 1983
Both between- and within-group differences in children ages nine to eleven identified as having reflective or impulsive cognitive tempos. Cognitive tempo was first assessed using Kagan's Matching Familiar Figures test, and children were later tested on the Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Conceptual Tempo, Difficulty Level, Individual Differences, Problem Solving

Weiner, Alan S.; Adams, Wayne V. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1974
Failure was hypothesized to be an antecedent of a reflective style of responding and frustration an antecedents of an impulsive style. Fourth grade children experienced either failure or frustration, and were then assessed with the Matching Familiar Figures Test. (SBT)
Descriptors: Academic Failure, Cognitive Style, Conceptual Tempo, Elementary School Students
Adams, Wayne V. – 1971
In an investigation of search strategies, it was predicted that reflective children would have developmentally more mature problem-solving strategies than impulsive children, and the presence of these strategies would be found in both 6- and 8-year-old subjects. From a sample given the Matching Familiar Figures Test, half of the 20 boys and 20…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Conceptual Tempo, Elementary School Students, Problem Solving

Brodzinsky, David M.; And Others – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1977
Fourth and seventh grade reflective, impulsive, fast/accurate, and slow/inaccurate subjects were asked to paraphrase the meaning of ambiguous sentences and then to indicate their meaning through the selection of pictures. Results indicated that reflective children were more successful in some settings than impulsive or slow/inaccurate children.…
Descriptors: Ambiguity, Cognitive Style, Conceptual Tempo, Elementary Education

Ault, Ruth L.; And Others – Child Development, 1976
Two statistical characteristics of the Matching Familiar Figures test which produce methodological problems in reflection-impulsivity research are discussed. (BRT)
Descriptors: Conceptual Tempo, Elementary Education, Research Methodology, Research Problems

Kagan, Jerome – Developmental Psychology, 1987
Reply by Jerome Kagan to a recent article by Block, Gjerde, and Block (1986) which questions the validity of the construct of reflection-impulsivity. Kagan alleges flaws in the logic of the authors' (Block, Gjerde, Block) position and in the inferences drawn from their data. (Author/RWB)
Descriptors: Character Recognition, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Conceptual Tempo