Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Source
Child Development | 51 |
Author
Zelniker, Tamar | 4 |
Ault, Ruth L. | 3 |
Brodzinsky, David M. | 3 |
Denney, Douglas R. | 2 |
Egeland, Byron | 2 |
Messer, Stanley B. | 2 |
Steinberg, Laurence | 2 |
Weiner, Alan S. | 2 |
Adams, Wayne V. | 1 |
Adult, Ruth L. | 1 |
Albert, Dustin | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 19 |
Reports - Research | 14 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 2 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 3 |
Location
France | 1 |
Israel | 1 |
Japan | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
United States | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Matching Familiar Figures Test | 10 |
Raven Progressive Matrices | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Hoyer, Roxane S.; Elshafei, Hesham; Hemmerlin, Julie; Bouet, Romain; Bidet-Caulet, Aurélie – Child Development, 2021
Distractibility is the propensity to behaviorally react to irrelevant information. Although children are more distractible the younger they are, the precise contribution of attentional and motor components to distractibility and their developmental trajectories have not been characterized yet. We used a new behavioral paradigm to identify the…
Descriptors: Child Development, Psychomotor Skills, Motor Development, Attention Control
Blakey, Emma; Visser, Ingmar; Carroll, Daniel J. – Child Development, 2016
Improvements in cognitive flexibility during the preschool years have been linked to developments in both working memory and inhibitory control, though the precise contribution of each remains unclear. In the current study, one hundred and twenty 2-, 3-, and 4-year-olds completed two rule-switching tasks. In one version, children switched rules in…
Descriptors: Executive Function, Preschool Children, Short Term Memory, Conceptual Tempo
Vandell, Deborah Lowe; Belsky, Jay; Burchinal, Margaret; Steinberg, Laurence; Vandergrift, Nathan – Child Development, 2010
Relations between nonrelative child care (birth to 4 1/2 years) and functioning at age 15 were examined (N = 1,364). Both quality and quantity of child care were linked to adolescent functioning. Effects were similar in size as those observed at younger ages. Higher quality care predicted higher cognitive-academic achievement at age 15, with…
Descriptors: Child Care, Adolescents, Academic Achievement, Antisocial Behavior
Albert, Dustin; Steinberg, Laurence – Child Development, 2011
The present study examined age differences in performance on the Tower of London, a measure of strategic planning, in a diverse sample of 890 individuals between the ages of 10 and 30. Although mature performance was attained by age 17 on relatively easy problems, performance on the hardest problems showed improvements into the early 20s.…
Descriptors: Strategic Planning, Self Control, Late Adolescents, Age Differences
Flynn, Emma; Whiten, Andrew – Child Development, 2012
In one of the first open diffusion experiments with young children, a tool-use task that afforded multiple methods to extract an enclosed reward and a child model habitually using one of these methods were introduced into different playgroups. Eighty-eight children, ranging in age from 2 years 8 months to 4 years 5 months, participated. Measures…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Socialization, Young Children, Verbal Ability
Snyder, James; Schrepferman, Lynn; McEachern, Amber; Barner, Stacy; Johnson, Kassy; Provines, Jessica – Child Development, 2008
The prospective relationships of conduct problems and peer coercion and deviancy training during kindergarten (mean age = 5.3 years) to overt and covert conduct problems in third-fourth grade were examined in a sample of 267 boys and girls. Coercion and deviancy training were distinct peer processes. Both were associated with earlier child conduct…
Descriptors: Peer Relationship, Verbal Ability, Grade 4, Grade 3
Laible, Deborah; Panfile, Tia; Makariev, Drika – Child Development, 2008
The goal of this study was to examine the links among attachment, child temperament, and the quality and frequency of mother-toddler conflict. Sixty-four mothers and children took part in a series of laboratory tasks when the child was 30 months of age and an audio-recorded home observation when the child was 36 months of age. All episodes of…
Descriptors: Mothers, Conflict, Toddlers, Parent Child Relationship

Fuqua, Robert W.; And Others – Child Development, 1975
Compares the performances of "impulsive" and "reflective" urban middle class children on a picture completion task designed to measure creativity. (CW)
Descriptors: Conceptual Tempo, Creativity, Preschool Children

Zelniker, Tamar; And Others – Child Development, 1977
Two types of problems similar to those on the Matching Familiar Figures Test were presented to 61 fourth graders. Results indicated that long or short decision time is a consistent trait related to preferred strategy of perceptual analysis, and accuracy depends on compatability between that strategy and specific task requirements. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Style, Conceptual Tempo, Elementary Education

Salkind, Neil J.; And Others – Child Development, 1978
Data on the Matching Familiar Figures test (MFF) for 760 Japanese, 2,676 American, and 1,619 Israeli children were used to examine cross-cultural differences in cognitive tempo. The data were compiled from other studies where the MFF was used as a classificatory variable. (JMB)
Descriptors: Conceptual Tempo, Cross Cultural Studies, Elementary School Students

Smith, J. David; Nelson, Deborah G. Kemler – Child Development, 1988
This study contrasted two possible relations between reflection-impulsivity and analytic or holistic modes of processing. Although impulsive children were more holistic in the classification task, they made more errors than reflectives on matching tests, regardless of whether the content favored holistic processing. (RH)
Descriptors: Children, Classification, Cognitive Processes, Conceptual Tempo

Wenckstern, Susanne; And Others – Child Development, 1984
Temporal stability of play behavior and its consistency among toys differing in complexity was assessed by observing 40 eight-month-old infants. The relationship of stability of play to temperament was examined. Findings support the idea that behavioral consistency in infancy is similar to that reported for older children. (RH)
Descriptors: Conceptual Tempo, Infant Behavior, Infants, Personality

Brodzinsky, David M.; Rightmyer, Jonathan – Child Development, 1976
Pleasure associated with cognitive mastery was studied in relation to conceptual tempo in third- and fourth-grade boys and girls. nAuthor/JMB)
Descriptors: Conceptual Tempo, Elementary Education, Emotional Response, Task Performance

Brodzinsky, David M. – Child Development, 1977
This study examined the role of conceptual tempo in 4th graders' comprehension and appreciation of verbal jokes containing various types of linguistic ambiguity. (JMB)
Descriptors: Ambiguity, Comprehension, Conceptual Tempo, Elementary School Students
Geary, David C.; Hoard, Mary K.; Byrd-Craven, Jennifer; Nugent, Lara; Numtee, Chattavee – Child Development, 2007
Using strict and lenient mathematics achievement cutoff scores to define a learning disability, respective groups of children who are math disabled (MLD, n = 15) and low achieving (LA, n = 44) were identified. These groups and a group of typically achieving (TA, n = 46) children were administered a battery of mathematical cognition, working…
Descriptors: Memory, Mathematics Achievement, Learning Disabilities, Disability Identification