Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 9 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 25 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 41 |
Descriptor
Conflict | 41 |
Cognitive Processes | 27 |
Reaction Time | 23 |
Foreign Countries | 15 |
Visual Stimuli | 14 |
College Students | 13 |
Interference (Learning) | 13 |
Task Analysis | 12 |
Color | 10 |
Cues | 8 |
Error Patterns | 8 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 41 |
Author
Bugg, Julie M. | 4 |
Dreisbach, Gesine | 2 |
Fischer, Rico | 2 |
Lupker, Stephen J. | 2 |
Spinelli, Giacomo | 2 |
Suh, Jihyun | 2 |
Weissman, Daniel H. | 2 |
Abrahamse, Elger | 1 |
Ackerman, Rakefet | 1 |
Ansorge, Ulrich | 1 |
Atas, Anne | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 41 |
Reports - Research | 38 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Numerical/Quantitative Data | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 21 |
Postsecondary Education | 18 |
Audience
Location
Germany | 4 |
Canada | 3 |
Belgium | 2 |
Israel | 2 |
Michigan | 2 |
Missouri | 2 |
Spain | 2 |
China (Beijing) | 1 |
Maryland | 1 |
Montana | 1 |
North Carolina | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 8 |
Raven Advanced Progressive… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Grant, Lauren D.; Weissman, Daniel H. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
Current views posit that forming and retrieving memories of ongoing events influences action control. However, the organizational structure of these memories, or event files, remains unclear. The "hierarchical coding view" posits a hierarchical structure, wherein task sets occupy a high level of the hierarchy. Here, the contents of an…
Descriptors: Memory, Generalization, Cognitive Processes, College Students
Luca Moretti; Iring Koch; Marco Steinhauser; Stefanie Schuch – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
In the present study, we used a modeling approach for measuring task conflict in task switching, assessing the probability of selecting the correct task via multinomial processing tree (MPT) modeling. With this method, task conflict and response conflict can be independently assessed as the probability of selecting the correct task and the…
Descriptors: Conflict, Persistence, Performance, Probability
Yang, Guochun; Xu, Honghui; Li, Zhenghan; Nan, Weizhi; Wu, Haiyan; Li, Qi; Liu, Xun – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2021
The congruence effect can be modulated by adjacent conflict conditions, producing the congruency sequence effect (CSE). However, many boundary conditions prevent the transfer of the cross-conflict CSE. A consensus has been achieved that the CSE reflects both top-down control and bottom-up associative learning, but neither perspective could…
Descriptors: Congruence (Psychology), Conflict, Cognitive Processes, Learning
Mittelstädt, Victor; Mackenzie, Ian Grant; Koob, Valentin; Janczyk, Markus – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
In the present study, we examined how the relevance of potentially distracting information modulates the interplay of target and distractor processing in conflict tasks. Specifically, we manipulated the degree to which distracting information is relevant for performing the overall task by varying the proportion of trials in which a response to the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Attention Control, Conflict, Task Analysis
Jiménez, Luis; Méndez, Cástor; Abrahamse, Elger; Braem, Senne – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2021
Humans are able to anticipate abstract task demands and prepare attentional sets accordingly. A popular method to study this ability is to include explicit cues that signal the required level of cognitive control in conflict tasks (e.g., whether or not word meaning will correspond to the task-relevant font color in a Stroop task). Here, we…
Descriptors: Congruence (Psychology), Cues, Cognitive Processes, College Students
Spinelli, Giacomo; Lupker, Stephen J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
In the standard Proportion-Congruent (PC) paradigm, performance is compared between a list containing mostly congruent (MC) stimuli (e.g., the word RED in the color red in the Stroop task; Stroop, 1935) and a list containing mostly incongruent (MI) stimuli (e.g., the word BLUE in red). The PC effect, the finding that the congruency effect (i.e.,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Psychology, Conflict, Cognitive Processes, Reaction Time
Cohen-Shikora, Emily R.; Suh, Jihyun; Bugg, Julie M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
In this article, we assess an alternative account of a key experimental pattern thought to index top-down control. The list-wide proportion congruence effect is the well-documented pattern whereby the congruency effect (i.e., Stroop effect) is attenuated in lists containing mostly incongruent trials relative to lists containing mostly congruent…
Descriptors: Congruence (Psychology), Reaction Time, Color, Conflict
Weissman, Daniel H. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
Although domain-specificity is prevalent in models of human cognition, its presence is not always easy to verify. For example, according to one prominent model, experiencing conflict from an incongruent distractor in a Stroop-like task triggers an upregulation of domain-specific control that facilitates the resolution of the same, but not a…
Descriptors: Color, Interference (Learning), Reaction Time, Visual Stimuli
Hedge, Craig; Powell, Georgina; Bompas, Aline; Sumner, Petroc – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2022
Response control or inhibition is one of the cornerstones of modern cognitive psychology, featuring prominently in theories of executive functioning and impulsive behavior. However, repeated failures to observe correlations between commonly applied tasks have led some theorists to question whether common response conflict processes even exist. A…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Processes, Meta Analysis
Suh, Jihyun; Bugg, Julie M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2021
Existing approaches in the literature on cognitive control in conflict tasks almost exclusively target the outcome of control (by comparing mean congruency effects) and not the processes that shape control. These approaches are limited in addressing a current theoretical issue--what contribution does learning make to adjustments in cognitive…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis, Conflict, Learning Processes
Spinelli, Giacomo; Lupker, Stephen J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2021
In the Stroop task, congruency effects (i.e., the color-naming latency difference between incongruent stimuli, e.g., the word BLUE written in the color red, and congruent stimuli, e.g., RED in red) are smaller in a list in which incongruent trials are frequent than in a list in which incongruent trials are infrequent. The traditional explanation…
Descriptors: Color, Interference (Learning), Visual Stimuli, Reaction Time
Dreisbach, Gesine; Fröber, Kerstin; Berger, Anja; Fischer, Rico – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
One prominent feature of adaptive cognition in humans is the ability to flexibly adjust to changing task demands. In this respect, context-specific proportion congruency (CSPC) effects describe the phenomenon that participants learn to adapt to contexts of frequently occurring conflicts even when the upcoming context cannot be anticipated. Here,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Adjustment (to Environment), Conflict, Context Effect
Hershman, Ronen; Henik, Avishai – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
It has been suggested that the Stroop task gives rise to 2 conflicts: the information conflict (color vs. word meaning) and the task conflict (name the color vs. read the word). However, behavioral indications for task conflict (reaction time [RT] congruent condition longer than RT neutral condition) appear under very restricted conditions. We…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Eye Movements, Color, Interference (Learning)
Fennell, Alex; Ratcliff, Roger – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
In the Stroop task, color words are presented in colored fonts and the task of the subject is to either name the word or name the color. If the word and font color are in agreement, then the stimulus is said to be congruent (e.g., RED in red font color); however, if the word and font color are not in agreement, the stimulus is said to be…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Modeling (Psychology), Interference (Learning), Responses
Bhatia, Sudeep – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
Conflict has been hypothesized to play a key role in recruiting deliberative processing in reasoning and judgment tasks. This claim suggests that changing the task so as to add incorrect heuristic responses that conflict with existing heuristic responses can make individuals less likely to respond heuristically and can increase response accuracy.…
Descriptors: Conflict, Bias, Heuristics, Accuracy