NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Wechsler Intelligence Scale…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 19 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Gürgil, Fitnat – International Journal of Progressive Education, 2021
This study researched the use of metaphor among students and teachers to determine the images they had in their minds regarding a specific set of developed countries, and in doing so, utilized a longitudinal design carried out over four stages between November 2015 and November 2018 within a large metropolitan city in Turkey. The study findings…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Concept Formation, Stereotypes, Positive Attitudes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Balta, Nuri; Cessna, Stephen G.; Kaliyeva, Assem – Physics Education, 2020
The Colorado learning attitudes about science survey (CLASS) was given to 308 high school physics students in Almaty, Kazakhstan, where physics is taught every year through middle and high school. Student responses were tested using several different latent factor models: two based on those published in the recent literature and two new models…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, High School Students, Physics, Secondary School Science
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Deckert, Matthias; Schmoeger, Michaela; Schaunig-Busch, Ines; Willinger, Ulrike – Journal of Child Language, 2019
Metaphor development in conjunction with verbal intelligence and linguistic competence in middle childhood and at the transition to early adolescence was investigated. 298 individuals between seven and ten years (chronological age) who attended grades two-four (mental age) were tested for metaphor processing by the Metaphoric Triads Task, for…
Descriptors: Verbal Ability, Linguistic Competence, Language Processing, Prediction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lourenco, Stella F.; Bonny, Justin W. – Developmental Science, 2017
A growing body of evidence suggests that non-symbolic representations of number, which humans share with nonhuman animals, are functionally related to uniquely human mathematical thought. Other research suggesting that numerical and non-numerical magnitudes not only share analog format but also form part of a general magnitude system raises…
Descriptors: Mathematics Skills, Skill Development, Correlation, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Shongwe, Benjamin – Journal of Urban Mathematics Education, 2022
The development of urban students' mathematical proving ability is a goal of several curricula frameworks, including some located in the southern hemisphere. However, in achieving this goal, most curriculum frameworks do so from a Western worldview, which is characterized by competition and the role of the individual. The purpose of this study was…
Descriptors: Disadvantaged, Grade 11, High School Students, Mathematics Instruction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Moran, Christine E.; Senseny, Karlen – Cogent Education, 2016
American students typically attend kindergarten at the chronological age (CA) of five and currently with the implementation of Common Core State Standards, there are expectations that children learn how to read in order to meet these academic standards, despite whether or not they are developmentally ready. This mixed methods study examined age…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Emergent Literacy, Mixed Methods Research, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Batchelor, Sophie; Keeble, Sarah; Gilmore, Camilla – Mathematical Thinking and Learning: An International Journal, 2015
When children learn to count, they map newly acquired symbolic representations of number onto preexisting nonsymbolic representations. The nature and timing of this mapping is currently unclear. Some researchers have suggested this mapping process helps children understand the cardinal principle of counting, while other evidence suggests that this…
Descriptors: Mathematics Skills, Preschool Children, Numeracy, Number Concepts
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Choi, Jinnie; Walters, Alyssa; Hoge, Pat – Online Learning, 2017
According to recent reports, K-12 full-time virtual school students have shown lower performance in math than their counterparts in brick-and-mortar schools. However, research is lacking in what kind of programmatic interventions virtual schools might be particularly well-suited to provide to improve math performance. Engaging students in…
Descriptors: Online Courses, Reflection, Virtual Classrooms, Educational Technology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sigelman, Carol K. – Applied Developmental Science, 2014
Guided by a naïve theories perspective on the development of thinking about disease, this study of 188 children aged 6 to 18 examined knowledge of HIV/AIDS causality and prevention using parallel measures derived from open-ended and structured interviews. Knowledge of both risk factors and prevention rules, as well as conceptual understanding of…
Descriptors: Beliefs, Prevention, Acquired Immunodeficiency Syndrome (AIDS), Communicable Diseases
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
van der Hoeven, Nienke; de Bot, Kees – Language Learning, 2012
This article reports on a study on learning new and relearning forgotten words of French as a foreign language in young (mean age 22.4), middle-aged (mean age 50.3), and elderly speakers (mean age 76.0). The three age groups performed similarly on relearning old words, but the younger learners were significantly better at learning new words. Data…
Descriptors: Older Adults, Age Differences, Short Term Memory, French
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brown, P. Margaret; Byrnes, Linda J.; Watson, Linda M.; Raban, Bridie – Journal of Early Childhood Research, 2013
This study investigated the relationships between children's home literacy environments and their early hypotheses about printed words in the year prior to entering school. There were 147 children (70 girls and 77 boys: mean age 57 months, range = 47-66 months, standard deviation = 4.5 months) in the study. Results showed that the children had…
Descriptors: Family Environment, Emergent Literacy, Printed Materials, Correlation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tsoi, Kwok Ho – Asia-Pacific Forum on Science Learning and Teaching, 2011
Global shark populations are seriously declining and many species are now threatened by anthropogenic stresses. Their extinction would cause devastating consequences to the marine biodiversity and ecosystems. However some children describe the sharks as bad guys, "we should kill them all!" Such children's view motivates my study…
Descriptors: Student Attitudes, Biodiversity, Information Sources, Animals
Thompson, Clarissa A.; Siegler, Robert S. – Grantee Submission, 2010
We investigated the relation between children's numerical-magnitude representations and their memory for numbers. Results of three experiments indicated that the more linear children's magnitude representations were, the more closely their memory of the numbers approximated the numbers presented. This relation was present for preschoolers and…
Descriptors: Teaching Methods, Memory, Numbers, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Nelson, Deborah G. Kemler; O'Neil, Kelly A.; Asher, Yvonne M. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2008
Two studies investigated the relationship between learning names and learning concepts in preschool children. More specifically, we focused on the relationship between learning the names and learning the intended functions of artifacts, given that the intended function of an artifact is generally thought to constitute core conceptual information…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Classification, Correlation, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cinan, Sevtap – Cognitive Development, 2006
This study examined developmental changes in concept formation, rule switching, and perseverative behaviors of children in the WCST by altering visual features of the test and using a new test score--the "zigzag" error score--which shows the number of shifts made between two incorrect concepts or rules. Instead of the original four 3-dimensional…
Descriptors: Concept Formation, Scores, Cognitive Development, Persistence
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2