Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 60 |
Descriptor
Cues | 211 |
Age Differences | 71 |
Recall (Psychology) | 54 |
Memory | 49 |
Cognitive Processes | 41 |
Young Children | 41 |
Children | 40 |
Preschool Children | 39 |
Elementary School Students | 36 |
Visual Stimuli | 35 |
Adults | 33 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental Child… | 211 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 166 |
Reports - Research | 151 |
Reports - Evaluative | 7 |
Reports - Descriptive | 3 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 14 |
Elementary Education | 12 |
Preschool Education | 4 |
Adult Education | 3 |
Kindergarten | 2 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 22 |
Location
Israel | 3 |
Canada | 2 |
Netherlands | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
Canada (Vancouver) | 1 |
France | 1 |
Malawi | 1 |
New Zealand | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
United Kingdom (England) | 1 |
West Germany | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Mean Length of Utterance | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 1 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 1 |
Sakkalou, Elena; Ellis-Davies, Kate; Fowler, Nia C.; Hilbrink, Elma E.; Gattis, Merideth – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Previous studies have reported that infants selectively reproduce observed actions and have argued that this selectivity reflects understanding of intentions and goals, or goal-directed imitation. We reasoned that if selective imitation of goal-directed actions reflects understanding of intentions, infants should demonstrate stability across…
Descriptors: Imitation, Infants, Goal Orientation, Experimental Psychology
Joh, Amy S.; Spivey, Leigh A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Spatial reasoning, a crucial skill for everyday actions, develops gradually during the first several years of childhood. Previous studies have shown that perceptual information and problem solving strategies are critical for successful spatial reasoning in young children. Here, we sought to link these two factors by examining children's use of…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Color, Cues, Spatial Ability
Betsch, Tilmann; Lang, Anna – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
We studied risky choices in preschoolers, elementary schoolers, and adults using an information board paradigm crossing two options with two cues that differ in their probability of making valid predictions (p = 0.50 vs. p = 0.83). We also varied the presence of normatively irrelevant information. Choice patterns indicate that preschoolers were…
Descriptors: Prediction, Probability, Preschool Children, Cues
Maionchi-Pino, Norbert; Taki, Yasuyuki; Yokoyama, Satoru; Magnan, Annie; Takahashi, Kei; Hashizume, Hiroshi; Ecalle, Jean; Kawashima, Ryuta – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
To date, the nature of the phonological deficit in developmental dyslexia is still debated. We concur with possible impairments in the representations of the universal phonological constraints that universally govern how phonemes co-occur as a source of this deficit. We were interested in whether-and how-dyslexic children have sensitivity to…
Descriptors: Reading, Age, Dyslexia, Syllables
Blake, Peter R.; Ganea, Patricia A.; Harris, Paul L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Children can identify owners either by seeing a person in possession of an object (a visual cue) and inferring that they are the owner or by hearing testimony about a claim of ownership (a verbal cue). A total of 391 children between 2.5 and 6 years of age were tested in three experiments assessing how children identify owners when these two cues…
Descriptors: Ownership, Toys, Cues, Social Experience
Chevalier, Nicolas; Wiebe, Sandra A.; Huber, Kristina L.; Espy, Kimberly Andrews – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
The current study addressed the role of switch detection in cognitive flexibility by testing the effect of transition cues (i.e., cues that directly signal the need to switch or maintain a given task goal) in a cued set-shifting paradigm at 5 years of age. Children performed better, especially on switch trials, when transition cues were combined…
Descriptors: Cues, Young Children, Cognitive Processes, Experimental Psychology
Kirkham, Natasha Z.; Richardson, Daniel C.; Wu, Rachel; Johnson, Scott P. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Dynamic spatial indexing is the ability to encode, remember, and track the location of complex events. For example, in a previous study, 6-month-old infants were familiarized to a toy making a particular sound in a particular location, and later they fixated that empty location when they heard the sound presented alone ("Journal of Experimental…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Processes, Spatial Ability, Acoustics
Cassia, Viola Macchi; Turati, Chiara; Schwarzer, Gudrun – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Sensitivity to variations in the spacing of features in faces and a class of nonface objects (i.e., frontal images of cars) was tested in 3- and 4-year-old children and adults using a delayed or simultaneous two-alternative forced choice matching-to-sample task. In the adults, detection of spacing information was robust against exemplar…
Descriptors: Cues, Memory, Preschool Children, Preschool Education
Blaye, Agnes; Chevalier, Nicolas – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
This study addressed the role of goal representation in preschoolers' inhibition and flexibility performance. A total of 56 4- and 5-year-olds were tested in an adapted version of the Shape School task where the difficulty of goal representation was manipulated by varying the degree of transparency of task cues. The findings showed that both age…
Descriptors: Cues, Inhibition, Goal Orientation, Task Analysis
Elsner, Birgit; Pfeifer, Caroline – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The impact of goal salience and verbal cues given by the model on 3- to 5-year-olds' reproduction of action components (movement or goal) was investigated in an imitation choice task. Preschoolers watched an experimenter moving a puppet up or down a ramp, terminating at one of two target objects. The target objects were either differently colored…
Descriptors: Cues, Imitation, Children, Preschool Education
Dack, Lisa Ain; Astington, Janet Wilde – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
It is widely accepted that adults show an advantage for deontic over epistemic reasoning. Two published studies (Cummins, 1996b; Harris and Nunez, 1996, Experiment 4) found evidence of this "deontic advantage" in preschool-aged children and are frequently cited as evidence that preschoolers show the same deontic advantage as adults. However,…
Descriptors: Cues, Context Effect, Thinking Skills, Epistemology
Ross, J.; Anderson, J. R.; Campbell, R. N. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
In adults, heightened self-awareness leads to adherence to socially valued norms, whereas lowered self-awareness is associated with antinormative behavior. Levels of self-awareness are influenced by environmental cues such as mirrors. Do situational changes in self-awareness also have an impact on preschoolers' self-regulation? Adherence to a…
Descriptors: Cues, Altruism, Metacognition, Preschool Children
Nelson, Nicole L.; Russell, James A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
To chart the developmental path of children's attribution of pride to others, we presented children (4 years 0 month to 11 years 11 months of age, N = 108) with video clips of head-and-face, body posture, and multi-cue (both head-and-face and body posture simultaneously) expressions that adults consider to convey pride. Across age groups, 4- and…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Child Development
Piasta, Shayne B.; Wagner, Richard K. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
Preschool-age children (N = 58) were randomly assigned to receive instruction in letter names and sounds, letter sounds only, or numbers (control). Multilevel modeling was used to examine letter name and sound learning as a function of instructional condition and characteristics of both letters and children. Specifically, learning was examined in…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Phonology, Alphabets, Cues
Bauer, Patricia J.; King, Jessica E.; Larkina, Marina; Varga, Nicole L.; White, Elizabeth A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Children build up knowledge about the world and also remember individual episodes. How individual episodes during which children learn new things become integrated with one another to form general knowledge is only beginning to be explored. Integration between separate episodes is called on in educational contexts and in everyday life as a major…
Descriptors: Problem Solving, Children, Research, Experiments