Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 19 |
Descriptor
Cues | 45 |
Discrimination Learning | 45 |
Stimuli | 23 |
Visual Stimuli | 16 |
Responses | 11 |
Memory | 9 |
Animals | 7 |
Learning Processes | 6 |
Autism | 5 |
Comparative Analysis | 5 |
Conditioning | 5 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Smeets, Paul M. | 4 |
Hayes, Brett K. | 2 |
Lee, Jessica C. | 2 |
Lovibond, Peter F. | 2 |
Aist, Eugene H. | 1 |
Anker, James M. | 1 |
Barnes, T. R. | 1 |
Beran, Michael J. | 1 |
Berch, Daniel B. | 1 |
Biddle, W. Barry | 1 |
Blanchette, Isabelle | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 28 |
Reports - Research | 23 |
Reports - Descriptive | 3 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
High Schools | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 3 |
Practitioners | 1 |
Teachers | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Expressive One Word Picture… | 1 |
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Lovibond, Peter F.; Lee, Jessica C.; Hayes, Brett K. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
Generalization of learning can arise from 2 distinct sources: failure to discriminate a novel test stimulus from the trained stimulus and active extrapolation from the trained stimulus to the test stimulus despite them being discriminable. We investigated these 2 processes in a predictive learning task by testing stimulus discriminability…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Discrimination Learning, Perception, Generalization
Lee, Jessica C.; Hayes, Brett K.; Lovibond, Peter F. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
Two experiments tested whether a peak-shifted generalization gradient could be explained by the averaging of distinct gradients displayed in subgroups reporting different generalization rules. Across experiments using a causal judgment task (Experiment 1) and a fear conditioning paradigm (Experiment 2), we found a close concordance between…
Descriptors: Generalization, Associative Learning, Discrimination Learning, Learning Theories
Ferrara, Nicole C.; Cullen, Patrick K.; Pullins, Shane P.; Rotondo, Elena K.; Helmstetter, Fred J. – Learning & Memory, 2017
Generalization of fear can involve abnormal responding to cues that signal safety and is common in people diagnosed with post-traumatic stress disorder. Differential auditory fear conditioning can be used as a tool to measure changes in fear discrimination and generalization. Most prior work in this area has focused on elevated amygdala activity…
Descriptors: Fear, Brain, Memory, Discrimination Learning
Kisamore, April N.; Karsten, Amanda M.; Mann, Charlotte C.; Conde, Kerry Ann – Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 2013
Axe (2008) speculated that some instances of intraverbal responding might be associated with limited or delayed acquisition because they require discrimination of multiple components of verbal stimuli. Past studies suggest that acquisition of responses under control of complex, multicomponent antecedent stimuli (e.g., conditional or compound…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Verbal Communication, Responses, Repetition
Reed, Sarah R.; Stahmer, Aubyn C.; Suhrheinrich, Jessica; Schreibman, Laura – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2013
Stimulus overselectivity is widely accepted as a stimulus control abnormality in autism spectrum disorders and subsets of other populations. Previous research has demonstrated a link between both chronological and mental age and overselectivity in typical development. However, the age at which children are developmentally ready to respond to…
Descriptors: Autism, Preschool Children, Cues, Mental Age
Troisi, Joseph R., II; Bryant, Erin; Kane, Jennifer – Psychological Record, 2012
Extinction and recovery of the discriminative stimulus effects of nicotine (0.3 mg/kg) was investigated with a devalued food reinforcer (rats sated). Sixteen rats were trained in a counterbalanced one manipulandum (nose-poke) drug discrimination procedure with the roles of nicotine and saline counterbalanced as S[superscript D] and S[superscript…
Descriptors: Therapy, Reinforcement, Smoking, Stimuli
Reynolds, Gemma; Reed, Phil – Learning and Motivation, 2011
Stimulus over-selectivity refers to behavior being controlled by one element of the environment at the expense of other equally salient aspects of the environment. This is a common problem for many individuals, including those with autism spectrum disorders, and learning difficulties, and presents a considerable problem for information processing…
Descriptors: Learning Problems, Cues, Autism, Discrimination Learning
Mok, Leh Woon; Estevez, Angeles F.; Overmier, J. Bruce – Psychological Record, 2010
The learning of the relations between discriminative stimuli, choice actions, and their outcomes can be characterized as conditional discriminative choice learning. Research shows that the technique of presenting unique outcomes for specific cued choices leads to faster and more accurate learning of such relations and has great potential to be…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Training Methods, Educational Researchers, Cognitive Development
Oliveira, Luis; Machado, Armando – Learning and Motivation, 2008
To test the assumptions of two models of timing, Scalar Expectancy Theory (SET) and Learning to Time (LeT), nine pigeons were exposed to two temporal discriminations, each signaled by a different cue. On half of the trials, pigeons learned to choose a red key after a 1.5-s horizontal bar and a green key after a 6-s horizontal bar; on the other…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Animals, Cues, Models
Winslow, James T.; Noble, Pamela L.; Davis, Michael – Learning & Memory, 2008
Individuals with anxiety disorders often do not respond to safety signals and hence continue to be afraid and anxious. Consequently, it is important to develop paradigms in animals that can directly study brain systems involved in learning about, and responding to, safety signals. We previously developed a discrimination procedure in rats of the…
Descriptors: Cues, Models, Safety, Discrimination Learning
Beran, Michael J.; Klein, Emily D.; Evans, Theodore A.; Chan, Betty; Flemming, Timothy M.; Harris, Emily H.; Washburn, David A.; Rumbaugh, Duane M. – Psychological Record, 2008
Learning styles in capuchin monkeys were assessed with a computerized reversal-learning task called the mediational paradigm. First, monkeys were trained to respond with 90% accuracy on a two-choice discrimination (A+B-). Then the authors examined differences in performance on three different types of reversal trials (A-B+, A-C+, B+C-), each of…
Descriptors: Cues, Teaching Methods, Prediction, Animals

Doan, Helen McK.; Cooper, Deborah L. – Child Development, 1971
Descriptors: Conditioning, Cues, Discrimination Learning, Kindergarten Children
Santi, Angelo; Van Rooyen, Patrick – Learning and Motivation, 2007
Two groups of rats were trained in a symbolic delayed matching-to-sample task with a 0-s delay to discriminate sample stimuli that consisted of sequences of tone bursts. For one group, sequences varied in number with total sequence duration controlled. For the other group, total sequence duration, sum of the tone durations, and sum of the gap…
Descriptors: Cues, Intervals, Figurative Language, Diagnostic Tests
Santangelo, Valerio; Spence, Charles – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2007
We compared the ability of auditory, visual, and audiovisual (bimodal) exogenous cues to capture visuo-spatial attention under conditions of no load versus high perceptual load. Participants had to discriminate the elevation (up vs. down) of visual targets preceded by either unimodal or bimodal cues under conditions of high perceptual load (in…
Descriptors: Cues, Attention Control, Attention, Visual Discrimination
Garotti, Marilice; De Rose, Julio C. – Psychological Record, 2007
Two experiments investigated baseline reviews as a relevant variable in reorganization of equivalence classes. After formation of three 4-member classes, participants learned reversals of baseline conditional discriminations and expanded the classes to 5 members each. In Experiment 1, 4 students responded on equivalence probes without baseline…
Descriptors: Cues, Operant Conditioning, Experiments, Stimuli