Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Depth Perception | 60 |
Visual Perception | 60 |
Visual Stimuli | 15 |
Perceptual Development | 14 |
Infants | 13 |
Spatial Ability | 13 |
Age Differences | 9 |
Cues | 9 |
Infant Behavior | 9 |
Visual Discrimination | 9 |
Cognitive Processes | 8 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Yonas, Albert | 4 |
Ball, William A. | 2 |
Bremner, J. Gavin | 2 |
Granrud, Carl E. | 2 |
Wagemans, Johan | 2 |
Allen, Sue | 1 |
Anderson, Barton L. | 1 |
Andreasen, Gillian | 1 |
Aslin, Richard N. | 1 |
Ball, William | 1 |
Batten, Annabel | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Higher Education | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 2 |
Audience
Practitioners | 5 |
Researchers | 4 |
Teachers | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Kok, Petrus Jacobus – African Journal of Research in Mathematics, Science and Technology Education, 2020
This research focused on first-year university students' visuospatial cognition in terms of producing three-dimensional (3D) representations of objects from two-dimensional (2D) views. The research was important since students often had difficulty with 2D to 3D transition activities. A synthesis from the literature established a 2D to 3D…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Preservice Teachers, Visual Perception, Spatial Ability
Sarah J. Anderson; Heather A. Jamniczky; Olave E. Krigolson; Sylvain P. Coderre; Kent G. Hecker – npj Science of Learning, 2019
Advances in computer visualization enabling both 2D and 3D representation have generated tools to aid perception of spatial relationships and provide a new forum for instructional design. A key knowledge gap is the lack of understanding of how the brain neurobiologically processes and learns from spatially presented content, and new quantitative…
Descriptors: Anatomy, Medicine, Brain, Neurology
Wagemans, Johan; Elder, James H.; Kubovy, Michael; Palmer, Stephen E.; Peterson, Mary A.; Singh, Manish; von der Heydt, Rudiger – Psychological Bulletin, 2012
In 1912, Max Wertheimer published his paper on phi motion, widely recognized as the start of Gestalt psychology. Because of its continued relevance in modern psychology, this centennial anniversary is an excellent opportunity to take stock of what Gestalt psychology has offered and how it has changed since its inception. We first introduce the key…
Descriptors: Psychology, Vision, Depth Perception, Visual Perception
Zacharis, Georgios S.; Mikropoulos, Tassos A.; Priovolou, Chryssi – Themes in Science and Technology Education, 2013
Previous studies report the involvement of specific brain activation in stereoscopic vision and the perception of depth information. This work presents the first comparative results of adult women on the effects of stereoscopic perception in three different static environments; a real, a two dimensional (2D) and a stereoscopic three dimensional…
Descriptors: Females, Simulated Environment, Neurosciences, Brain
Kogo, Naoki; Strecha, Christoph; Van Gool, Luc; Wagemans, Johan – Psychological Review, 2010
Human visual perception is a fundamentally relational process: Lightness perception depends on luminance ratios, and depth perception depends on occlusion (difference of depth) cues. Neurons in low-level visual cortex are sensitive to the difference (but not the value itself) of signals, and these differences have to be used to reconstruct the…
Descriptors: Cues, Depth Perception, Mathematical Models, Visual Perception
Ono, Fuminori; Kitazawa, Shigeru – Cognition, 2010
The present study examined the effect of perceived motion-in-depth on temporal interval perception. We required subjects to estimate the length of a short empty interval starting from the offset of a first marker and ending with the onset of a second marker. The size of the markers was manipulated so that the subjects perceived a visual object as…
Descriptors: Intervals, Motion, Visual Perception, Time Perspective
Martin, Santiago; Rubio, Ramon – Computers & Education, 2009
The explanation of certain 3D concepts is based on 2D drawings. These drawings should contain certain depth cues, such as perspective and overlapping. Until recently, parallax has not been used as a depth cue. Nevertheless, new technologies allow it to be incorporated. This forms the background to our study of the design of interactive educational…
Descriptors: Cues, Depth Perception, Spatial Ability, Technical Education
O'Reilly, Michelle; Vollmer, Brigitte; Vargha-Khadem, Faraneh; Neville, Brian; Connelly, Alan; Wyatt, John; Timms, Chris; De Haan, Michelle – Developmental Science, 2010
Many studies report chronic deficits in visual processing in children born preterm. We investigated whether functional abnormalities in visual processing exist in children born preterm but without major neuromotor impairment (i.e. cerebral palsy). Twelve such children (less than 33 weeks gestation or birthweight less than 1000 g) without major…
Descriptors: Cognitive Tests, Premature Infants, Visual Acuity, Depth Perception
Anderson, Barton L. – Psychological Review, 2007
There has been a growing interest in understanding the computations involved in the processes underlying visual segmentation and interpolation in conditions of occlusion. P. J. Kellman, P. Garrigan, T. F. Shipley, and B. P. Keane and M. K. Albert defended the view that identical contour interpolation mechanisms underlie modal and amodal…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Phenomenology, Lighting, Models

Granrud, Carl E.; And Others – Child Development, 1984
A total of 20 infants either five or seven months of age viewed computer-generated random-lot displays in which accretion and deletion of texture provided the only information for contours. Infants of both age groups showed significant preferences to reach for the apparently nearer regions in the displays. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Depth Perception, Infants, Spatial Ability, Visual Perception

Dev, P. – International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 1975
Descriptors: Depth Perception, Models, Neurological Organization, Perception

Wilcox, Barbara Lee; Teghtsoonian, Martha – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1971
A developmental study of the relation between apparent size and pictorial depth demonstrated the usefulness of operant techniques in establishing equivalent differential responding and interesting stimulus equivalence in adults, 9- and 3-year-olds. (Author/AJ)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Cues

Gordon, F. Robert; Yonas, Albert – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1976
In order to study infants' sensitivity to binocular information for depth, 11 infants, 20 to 26 weeks of age, were presented with real and stereoscopically projected virtual objects at three distances, and the infants' reaching behavior was videotaped. (Author/SB)
Descriptors: Depth Perception, Infants, Perceptual Development, Research
Cook, Michael; Gillam, Barbara – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2004
Quantitative depth based on binocular resolution of visibility constraints is demonstrated in a novel stereogram representing an object, visible to 1 eye only, and seen through an aperture or camouflaged against a background. The monocular region in the display is attached to the binocular region, so that the stereogram represents an object which…
Descriptors: Depth Perception, Visual Perception, Visual Stimuli, Psychological Studies
Ball, William A. – 1977
In this study examining infants' responses to optical expansion, 18 infants between 36 and 61 days old watched expanding shadows that differed in the terminal location of the center of expansion and the number of dimensions undergoing change. Babies consistently rotated their heads upward during expansion of a closed figure when the center of…
Descriptors: Depth Perception, Infant Behavior, Infants, Perceptual Development