NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 17 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gava, Lucia; Valenza, Eloisa; Turati, Chiara – Child Development, 2009
Five experiments examined 79 newborns' ability to discriminate and categorize a spatial relation, defined by the left-right spatial position of a blinking object-target with respect to a vertical landmark-bar. Three-day-old infants discriminated the up versus low position of an object located on the same side of the landmark-bar (Experiment 1) and…
Descriptors: Neonates, Spatial Ability, Visual Stimuli, Visual Discrimination
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Quinn, Paul C. – Child Development, 2008
J. Kagan (2008) urges contemporary developmentalists to (a) be cautious when attributing conceptual knowledge to infants based on looking-time performance, (b) constrain their interpretation of infant performance with multiple methodologies, and (c) reconsider the possibility that qualitative development may be the path by which perceptual infants…
Descriptors: Infants, Child Development, Infant Behavior, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bertenthal, Bennett I.; Fischer, Kurt W. – Child Development, 1983
Three experiments tested whether 12- to 24-month-old children showed systematic search, persistence, and/or end-screen search in the invisible-displacement task. A fourth study tested whether end-screen search resulted from seeing the experimenter move his hand through the series of screens. (RH)
Descriptors: Behavior Patterns, Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Furrow, David; James, Patricia – Child Development, 1985
When not socially engaged, children showed a significantly greater percentage of reoriented attention during vocalizing than nonvocalizing periods. Findings confirm the existence of an attention/vocalization relation and are consonant with Greenfield's predictions about the nature of this relation. The relation held equally for prelinguistic and…
Descriptors: Attention, Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Jones, Susan S. – Child Development, 1996
Three studies examined the tongue protrusion (TP) behaviors of young infants in response to visual stimuli. Infants produced TPs in response to objects within reach before but not after the onset of reaching behavior. The results suggest that infants' TPs in response to a tongue-protruding adult reflect very early attempts at oral exploration of…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Body Language, Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fenson, Larry; Ramsay, Douglas S. – Child Development, 1980
Two studies (one cross-sectional and one longitudinal) examined the relation between two developmental trends in play during the second year of life: the emergence of decentered acts, and the integration of separate acts into simple sequences. Subjects were infants aged 13, 19, and 24 months. (CM)
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Comprehension, Developmental Stages, Egocentrism
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Goldfield, Eugene C.; And Others – Child Development, 1993
A theory of infant skill acquisition was supported by observations, over a six-week period, of the bouncing activity of eight infants while they were supported in a harness assembly. Observed three stages of activity: an initial assembly stage, when movement was irregular and variable in period; a tuning phase of more periodic movement; a final…
Descriptors: Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Infants, Longitudinal Studies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ganea, Patricia A. – Child Development, 2005
How do infants come to understand references to absent objects? 14-month-old infants first learned a name for a novel toy, which was then placed out of view. The infants who listened to a story mentioning the nonvisible object, looked, pointed, and searched for it more often than did infants who heard a story using a different name. Their behavior…
Descriptors: Toys, Infants, Context Effect, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Namy, Laura L.; Waxman, Sandra R. – Child Development, 1998
Three experiments examined the relation between language acquisition and other symbolic abilities in 18- and 26-month-olds. Found that 18-month-olds spontaneously interpreted gestures, like words, as names for object categories. At 26 months, they spontaneously interpreted words as names and novel gestures as names only when given additional…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bremner, J. Gavin; Johnson, Scott P.; Slater, Alan; Mason, Uschi; Foster, Kirsty; Cheshire, Andrea; Spring, Joanne – Child Development, 2005
When an object moves behind an occluder and re-emerges, 4-month-old infants perceive trajectory continuity only when the occluder is narrow, raising the question of whether time or distance out of sight is the important constraining variable. One hundred and forty 4-month-olds were tested in five experiments aimed to disambiguate time and distance…
Descriptors: Infants, Infant Behavior, Perceptual Development, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Sroufe, L. Alan – Child Development, 1977
This article reviews the literature on infants' reactions to strangers, focusing on issues of assessment, reliability, and stability. (JMB)
Descriptors: Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Infants, Literature Reviews
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Thomas, David G.; And Others – Child Development, 1981
Seeks to determine (1) whether 11- and 13-month-old infants directed their eye fixations to the referent of an object word said by the mother, and (2) whether there was a developmental shift in responding to object words at these two ages. Controls were set for response bias, stimulus preference, and maternal cuing. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Comprehension, Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
St James-Roberts, Ian; Plewis, Ian – Child Development, 1996
Used multilevel analyses to examined the amounts of time infants spent asleep, awake, content, feeding, fussing, and crying at 2, 6, 12, and 40 weeks of age. Found that day-to-day fluctuations accounted for between 44 and 53% of the variance in amounts of time sleeping, fussing, and crying. (MDM)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Crying, Developmental Stages, Eating Habits
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Mosier, Christine E.; Rogoff, Barbara – Child Development, 1994
Sixty-four mother-infant pairs were videotaped during structured episodes in which the mother challenged the infant to use her instrumentally to get access to or to operate a toy. At age 6 months the infants could use their mothers instrumentally in 36% of the episodes, increasing to 67% at 9 months and 78% at 13 months. (MDM)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Development, Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bronson, Gordon W. – Child Development, 1994
Examined the visual scanning patterns of infants ages 6, 10, and 13 weeks who viewed static geometric figures. Measures of fixation dwell-times, saccade lengths, and the choices and sequences of saccadic targets revealed that, although younger infants demonstrated salience-guided scanning behavior, older infants increasingly utilized volitional…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Developmental Stages, Eye Fixations, Individual Power
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2