NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Researchers1
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 86 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Piesie A. G. Asuako; Robert Stojan; Otmar Bock; Melanie Mack; Claudia Voelcker-Rehage – Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2025
It is well established that performing multiple tasks simultaneously (dual-tasking) or sequentially (task-switching) degrades performance on one or both tasks. However, it is unknown whether task-switching adds to the effects of dual-tasking in a single setup. We investigated this in a simulated everyday-like car driving scenario. We expected an…
Descriptors: Task Analysis, Time Management, Motor Vehicles, Performance
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Patrick V. Barnwell; Jake A. Rattigan; Kyle T. Brennan; Erick J. Fedorenko; Richard J. Contrada – Journal of American College Health, 2025
Objectives: To examine college students' conflicting COVID-19 information exposure, information-seeking, concern, and cognitive functioning. Participants: 179 undergraduates were recruited in March-April 2020, and 220 in September 2020 (Samples 1 and 2, respectively). Methods: Students completed the Attention Network Test, NASA Task Load Index,…
Descriptors: COVID-19, Pandemics, Undergraduate Students, Information Seeking
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Élisabeth Bélanger; Lorie-Marlène Brault Foisy; Steve Masson – International Journal of Research & Method in Education, 2025
The main objective of this methodological article is to discuss the contribution of response times as a tool in education research. The use of response times in research is largely a legacy of the work carried out in cognitive psychology, which has made it possible to describe the cognitive processes involved in information processing. In…
Descriptors: Educational Research, Reaction Time, Cognitive Processes, Research Methodology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kelsey J. MacKay; Filip Germeys; Wim Van Dooren; Lieven Verschaffel; Koen Luwel – Educational Studies in Mathematics, 2025
Rational numbers, such as fractions and decimals, are harder to understand than natural numbers. Moreover, individuals struggle with fractions more than with decimals. The present study sought to disentangle the extent to which two potential sources of difficulty affect secondary-school students' numerical magnitude understanding: number type…
Descriptors: Number Concepts, Numeracy, Secondary School Mathematics, Secondary School Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stefan Vermeent; Ethan S. Young; Meriah L. DeJoseph; Anna-Lena Schubert; Willem E. Frankenhuis – Developmental Science, 2024
Childhood adversity can lead to cognitive deficits or enhancements, depending on many factors. Though progress has been made, two challenges prevent us from integrating and better understanding these patterns. First, studies commonly use and interpret raw performance differences, such as response times, which conflate different stages of cognitive…
Descriptors: Early Experience, Trauma, Cognitive Processes, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Jianqiang Ye; Junhua Gao; Tingting Lin; Kun He; Dimei Chen – Journal of Baltic Science Education, 2025
This study explored the impact of oxidation-reduction reaction problem difficulty on university students' cognitive load using event-related potentials (ERPs). Forty-eight balanced low and high difficulty problems were designed. Fifteen undergraduate students majoring in chemistry (8 females and 7 males) participated in the study. Results…
Descriptors: Science Instruction, Undergraduate Students, Chemistry, Difficulty Level
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Peper, Phil; Alakbarova, Durna; Ball, B. Hunter – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
Prospective memory (PM) refers to the ability to remember to complete a task at the appropriate moment in the future. Past research has found reminders can improve PM performance in both laboratory and naturalistic settings, but few projects have examined the circumstances when and what types of reminders are most beneficial. Three experiments in…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level, Memory, Cues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yi Ding; Qian Wang; Ru-De Liu; Jolene Trimm; Jiayi Wang; Shu Feng; Wei Hong; Xian-Tong Yang – SAGE Open, 2024
The paper examined the relations among problem solving, automaticity, and working memory load (WML) by changing the difficulty level of task characteristics through two applications. In Study 1, involving 68 engineering students, a 2 (automaticity) x 2 (WML) design was utilized for arithmetic problems. In Study 2, involving 76 engineering…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level, Problem Solving
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
De Keersmaeker, Karen; Van Hoof, Jo; Van Dooren, Wim – European Journal of Psychology of Education, 2023
Processing rational numbers is difficult for many children. The natural number bias is one possible explanation for why children struggle with rational numbers. It refers to the tendency to overgeneralize the properties of natural numbers. In this study, it is argued that in order to be successful in rational number tasks, individuals need to…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Elementary School Mathematics, Mathematical Concepts, Number Concepts
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gross, Marina P.; Dobbins, Ian G. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2021
Under cognitive load theory, time pressure/urgency-induced arousal is a major contributor to pupil dilation during cognition. However, pupillometric encoding studies have failed to consider the possible role of time pressure/urgency effects, instead often assuming that encoding dilations directly reflect encoding strength. To isolate possible…
Descriptors: Memory, Physiology, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Xiang, Ming; Kramer, Alex; Nordmeyer, Ann E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
In sentence comprehension, negative sentences tend to elicit more processing cost than affirmative sentences. A growing body of work has shown that pragmatic context is an important factor that contributes to negation comprehension cost. The nature of this pragmatic effect, however, is yet to be determined. In 4 behavioral experiments, the current…
Descriptors: Pragmatics, Sentences, Comprehension, Expectation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hostetler, Kirsten; Luo, Tian – Educational Technology Research and Development, 2021
This study examined how instructional librarians can incorporate principles from cognitive load theory to engage students in research and improve learning outcomes for a common library instructional delivery model. The study employed a between-subjects, quasi-experimental design to compare how instructional sessions would impact information…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level, Information Literacy, Library Instruction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Joseph, Tanya N.; Morey, Candice C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2022
Previous work with complex memory span tasks, in which simple choice decisions are imposed between presentations of to-be-remembered items, shows that these secondary tasks reduce memory span. It is less clear how reconfiguring and maintaining various amounts of information affects decision speeds. We introduced preliminary "lead-in"…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level, Reaction Time
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kaur, Shraddha; Norris, Dennis G.; Gathercole, Susan E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
Running span can be performed by either passively listening to memory items or actively updating the target set. Previous research suggests that the active updating process is demanding and time consuming and is favored at slow rates of presentation while the passive strategy is employed at fast rates. Two experiments examined the time course of…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Recall (Psychology), Reaction Time, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bird, Lisa; Gretton, Matthew; Cockerell, Robert; Heathcote, Andrew – Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2019
Lying is assumed to increase cognitive load, and it has been shown to slow response times to simple questions. We employed a dual-task methodology, the detection response task (DRT), to assess cognitive load in telling narrative lies in a live, open-question interview. The DRT requires participants to press a button in response to a tactile…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level, Deception, Reaction Time
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6