Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 5 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 11 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 20 |
Descriptor
Conflict | 21 |
Error Patterns | 21 |
Cognitive Processes | 14 |
Foreign Countries | 10 |
Visual Stimuli | 10 |
Reaction Time | 9 |
College Students | 6 |
Correlation | 5 |
Task Analysis | 5 |
Comparative Analysis | 4 |
Interference (Learning) | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Lupker, Stephen J. | 2 |
Spinelli, Giacomo | 2 |
Berger, Anja | 1 |
Boer, Frits | 1 |
Botvinick, Matthew M. | 1 |
Bugg, Julie M. | 1 |
Ceuleers, Evy | 1 |
Chwilla, Dorothee J. | 1 |
Clawson, Ann | 1 |
Clayson, Peter E. | 1 |
Cohen, Jonathan D. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 20 |
Reports - Research | 17 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Collected Works - Proceedings | 1 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 9 |
Postsecondary Education | 8 |
Secondary Education | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 1 |
High Schools | 1 |
Audience
Location
Canada | 2 |
Germany | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
Belgium | 1 |
Canada (Victoria) | 1 |
China | 1 |
Czech Republic | 1 |
Greece | 1 |
Indonesia | 1 |
Japan | 1 |
Missouri | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 3 |
Autism Diagnostic Observation… | 1 |
Raven Advanced Progressive… | 1 |
Wechsler Adult Intelligence… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Patrick V. Barnwell; Jake A. Rattigan; Kyle T. Brennan; Erick J. Fedorenko; Richard J. Contrada – Journal of American College Health, 2025
Objectives: To examine college students' conflicting COVID-19 information exposure, information-seeking, concern, and cognitive functioning. Participants: 179 undergraduates were recruited in March-April 2020, and 220 in September 2020 (Samples 1 and 2, respectively). Methods: Students completed the Attention Network Test, NASA Task Load Index,…
Descriptors: COVID-19, Pandemics, Undergraduate Students, Information Seeking
Iwan Setiawan HR; Purwanto; Sukoriyanto; I Nengah Parta – International Journal of Educational Methodology, 2023
This study aims to evaluate cognitive conflict in constructing mathematical concepts, based on thinking errors. The data were collected through observations of words or sentences, leading to the derivation of qualitative outputs. Furthermore, the results showed that the two selected subjects experienced cognitive conflicts regarding thinking…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Conflict, Thinking Skills, Error Patterns
Spinelli, Giacomo; Lupker, Stephen J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
In the standard Proportion-Congruent (PC) paradigm, performance is compared between a list containing mostly congruent (MC) stimuli (e.g., the word RED in the color red in the Stroop task; Stroop, 1935) and a list containing mostly incongruent (MI) stimuli (e.g., the word BLUE in red). The PC effect, the finding that the congruency effect (i.e.,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Psychology, Conflict, Cognitive Processes, Reaction Time
Hall, Amy Cox – Evidence & Policy: A Journal of Research, Debate and Practice, 2019
In this practice paper I examine the evaluation of one project and ask whether or not the evaluation failed. I suggest that because I was unaware of the larger connections between the grant agency and the grantee I made tactical errors in the presentation of findings, which resulted in not simply disagreement but anger and resentment. I conclude…
Descriptors: Program Evaluation, Error Patterns, Psychological Patterns, Evaluators
Lu, Jijian; Tao, Yan; Xu, Jinghao; Stephens, Max – Interactive Learning Environments, 2023
This study is the extension of our previous visualizing study on the commognition processes in computer-supported one-to-one tutoring. With the help of the scale of commognitive responsibility score, we found that the main triggers of the commognition process shift are the positive transfer of knowledge and cognitive conflict. On the basis of…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Computer Assisted Instruction, Tutoring, Artificial Intelligence
Spinelli, Giacomo; Lupker, Stephen J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2021
In the Stroop task, congruency effects (i.e., the color-naming latency difference between incongruent stimuli, e.g., the word BLUE written in the color red, and congruent stimuli, e.g., RED in red) are smaller in a list in which incongruent trials are frequent than in a list in which incongruent trials are infrequent. The traditional explanation…
Descriptors: Color, Interference (Learning), Visual Stimuli, Reaction Time
Dreisbach, Gesine; Fröber, Kerstin; Berger, Anja; Fischer, Rico – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
One prominent feature of adaptive cognition in humans is the ability to flexibly adjust to changing task demands. In this respect, context-specific proportion congruency (CSPC) effects describe the phenomenon that participants learn to adapt to contexts of frequently occurring conflicts even when the upcoming context cannot be anticipated. Here,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Adjustment (to Environment), Conflict, Context Effect
Schuch, Stefanie; Pütz, Sebastian – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
The present study investigated the influence of mood state (positive vs. negative) on the cognitive control process of conflict adaptation. A task-switching paradigm was applied, allowing to assess conflict adaptation both within tasks and across tasks. A success-failure manipulation was applied for mood induction. Within-task conflict adaptation…
Descriptors: Task Analysis, Positive Attitudes, Negative Attitudes, Conflict
Hubbard, Jason; Kuhns, David; Schäfer, Theo A. J.; Mayr, Ulrich – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
Conflict-adaptation effects (i.e., reduced response-time costs on high-conflict trials following high-conflict trials) supposedly represent our cognitive system's ability to regulate itself according to current processing demands. However, currently it is not clear whether these effects reflect conflict-triggered, active regulation, or passive…
Descriptors: Conflict, Adjustment (to Environment), Reaction Time, Cognitive Processes
Thoma, Athina; Nardi, Elena – International Journal of Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education, 2018
We explore the transition from school to university through a commognitive (Sfard 2008) analysis of twenty-two students' examination scripts from the end of year examination of a first year, year-long module on Sets, Numbers, Proofs and Probability in a UK mathematics department. Our analysis of the scripts relies on a preliminary analysis of the…
Descriptors: Secondary School Mathematics, College Mathematics, Foreign Countries, Mathematical Concepts
Lubin, Amélie; Simon, Grégory; Houdé, Olivier; De Neys, Wim – ZDM: The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 2015
The acquisition of number conservation is a critical step in children's numerical and mathematical development. Classic developmental studies have established that children's number conservation is often biased by misleading intuitions. However, the precise nature of these conservation errors is not clear. A key question is whether conservation…
Descriptors: Mathematics Education, Inhibition, Numeracy, Number Concepts
Lever, Anne G.; Ridderinkhof, K. Richard; Marsman, Maarten; Geurts, Hilde M. – Developmental Psychology, 2017
As a large heterogeneity is observed across studies on interference control in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), research may benefit from the use of a cognitive framework that models specific processes underlying reactive and proactive control of interference. Reactive control refers to the expression and suppression of responses and proactive…
Descriptors: Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Responses, Self Control
Meier, Matt E.; Kane, Michael J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
Three experiments examined the relation between working memory capacity (WMC) and 2 different forms of cognitive conflict: stimulus-stimulus (S-S) and stimulus-response (S-R) interference. Our goal was to test whether WMC's relation to conflict-task performance is mediated by stimulus-identification processes (captured by S-S conflict),…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Short Term Memory, Executive Function, Cognitive Processes
Woumans, Evy; Ceuleers, Evy; Van der Linden, Lize; Szmalec, Arnaud; Duyck, Wouter – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
The present study explored the relation between language control and nonverbal cognitive control in different bilingual populations. We compared monolinguals, Dutch-French unbalanced bilinguals, balanced bilinguals, and interpreters on the Simon task (Simon & Rudell, 1967) and the Attention Network Test (ANT; Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz,…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Verbal Ability, Nonverbal Ability, Cognitive Processes
Bugg, Julie M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
The conflict monitoring account posits that globally high levels of conflict trigger engagement of top-down control; however, recent findings point to the mercurial nature of top-down control in high conflict contexts. The current study examined the potential moderating effect of associative learning on conflict-triggered top-down control…
Descriptors: Conflict, Experimental Psychology, Associative Learning, Hypothesis Testing
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2