Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Evaluation Methods | 9 |
Models | 9 |
Attention | 4 |
Cognitive Processes | 4 |
Universities | 4 |
Foreign Countries | 3 |
Task Analysis | 3 |
Adults | 2 |
Attention Control | 2 |
Comparative Analysis | 2 |
Experimental Psychology | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 9 |
Author
Ambadar, Zara | 1 |
Bialkova, Svetlana | 1 |
Blake, Randolph | 1 |
Boker, Steven M. | 1 |
Bootsma, Reinoud J. | 1 |
Boy, Frederic | 1 |
Brascamp, Jan W. | 1 |
Brick, Timothy R. | 1 |
Caparos, Serge | 1 |
Cohen, Asher | 1 |
Cohn, Jeffrey F. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 5 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 5 |
Adult Education | 2 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Linnell, Karina J.; Caparos, Serge – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Caparos and Linnell (2009, 2010) used a variable-separation flanker paradigm to show that (a) when cognitive load is low, increasing perceptual load causes spatial attention to focus and (b) when perceptual load is high, decreasing cognitive load causes spatial attention to focus. Here, we tested whether the effects of perceptual and cognitive…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Attention Control, Attention, Cognitive Processes
Oberauer, Klaus; Bialkova, Svetlana – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Six young adults practiced for 36 sessions on a working-memory updating task in which 2 digits and 2 spatial positions were continuously updated. Participants either did 1 updating operation at a time, or attempted 1 numerical and 1 spatial operation at the same time. In contrast to previous research using the same paradigm with a single digit and…
Descriptors: Attention, Young Adults, Short Term Memory, Costs
Brascamp, Jan W.; Blake, Randolph; Kristjansson, Arni – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
With attention and eye-movements humans orient to targets of interest. This orienting occurs faster when the same target repeats: priming of pop-out (PoP). While reaction times (RTs) can be important, PoP's real function could be to steer "where" to orient, a possibility underexposed in many current paradigms, as these predesignate a target to…
Descriptors: Priming, Reaction Time, Models, Evaluation Methods
Boy, Frederic; Sumner, Petroc – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
When associations between certain visual stimuli and particular actions are learned, those stimuli become capable of automatically and unconsciously activating their associated action plans. Such sensorimotor priming is assumed to be fundamental for efficient responses, and can be reliably measured in masked prime studies even when the primes are…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Processes, Organizations (Groups), Prediction
Magen, Hagit; Cohen, Asher – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
The Dimension Action (DA) model asserts that the visual system is modular, and that each task involves multiple-response mechanisms rather than a unitary-response selection mechanism. The model has been supported by evidence from single-task interference paradigms. We use the psychological refractory period paradigm and show that dual-task…
Descriptors: Models, Visual Perception, Task Analysis, Experiments
Boker, Steven M.; Cohn, Jeffrey F.; Theobald, Barry-John; Matthews, Iain; Mangini, Michael; Spies, Jeffrey R.; Ambadar, Zara; Brick, Timothy R. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
During conversation, women tend to nod their heads more frequently and more vigorously than men. An individual speaking with a woman tends to nod his or her head more than when speaking with a man. Is this due to social expectation or due to coupled motion dynamics between the speakers? We present a novel methodology that allows us to randomly…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Speech Communication, Motion, Sexual Identity
Leighton, Jane; Heyes, Cecilia – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
The effector dependence of automatic imitation was investigated using a stimulus-response compatibility (SRC) procedure during which participants were required to make an open or closed response with their hand or their mouth. The correct response for each trial was indicated by a pair of letters in Experiments 1 and 2 and by a colored square in…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Imitation, Geometric Concepts, Investigations
Bootsma, Reinoud J.; Fernandez, Laure; Morice, Antoine H. P.; Montagne, Gilles – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Using a two-step approach, Van Soest et al. (2010) recently questioned the pertinence of the conclusions drawn by Bootsma and Van Wieringen (1990) with respect to the visual regulation of an exemplary rapid interceptive action: the attacking forehand drive in table tennis. In the first step, they experimentally compared the movement behaviors of…
Descriptors: Architecture, Racquet Sports, Human Body, Motion
Visser, Troy A. W. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2007
When observers are presented with 2 targets in rapid succession, identification of the 1st is highly accurate, whereas identification of the 2nd is impaired at brief intertarget intervals (i.e., 200-500 ms). This 2nd-target deficit is known as the attentional blink (AB). According to bottleneck models, the AB arises because attending to the 1st…
Descriptors: Intervals, Identification, Attention, Eye Movements