Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 2 |
Experiments | 2 |
Intervals | 2 |
Older Adults | 2 |
Age Groups | 1 |
Attitude Change | 1 |
Beliefs | 1 |
Cognitive Processes | 1 |
Comparative Analysis | 1 |
Comparative Testing | 1 |
Error Correction | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Ecker, Ullrich K. H. | 1 |
Kliegl, Reinhold | 1 |
Krampe, Ralf Th. | 1 |
Lewandowsky, Stephan | 1 |
Mayr, Ulrich | 1 |
Swire, Briony | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Reports - Research | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Australia | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Swire, Briony; Ecker, Ullrich K. H.; Lewandowsky, Stephan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
People frequently continue to use inaccurate information in their reasoning even after a credible retraction has been presented. This phenomenon is often referred to as the continued influence effect of misinformation. The repetition of the original misconception within a retraction could contribute to this phenomenon, as it could inadvertently…
Descriptors: Information Utilization, Familiarity, Error Correction, Misconceptions
Krampe, Ralf Th.; Mayr, Ulrich; Kliegl, Reinhold – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2005
The authors demonstrate that the timing and sequencing of target durations require low-level timing and executive control. Sixteen young (M-sub(age) = 19 years) and 16 older (M-sub(age) = 70 years) adults participated in 2 experiments. In Experiment 1, individual mean-variance functions for low-level timing (isochronous tapping) and the sequencing…
Descriptors: Young Adults, Older Adults, Motion, Psychomotor Skills