Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 32 |
Descriptor
Experiments | 36 |
Eye Movements | 36 |
Visual Perception | 36 |
Visual Stimuli | 14 |
Human Body | 13 |
Foreign Countries | 10 |
Cognitive Processes | 8 |
College Students | 7 |
Attention | 6 |
Comparative Analysis | 6 |
Reaction Time | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Rayner, Keith | 3 |
Becker, Stefanie I. | 2 |
Kliegl, Reinhold | 2 |
Leo, Irene | 2 |
Luck, Steven J. | 2 |
Pollatsek, Alexander | 2 |
Allison, Robert S. | 1 |
Alsius, Agnès | 1 |
Angele, Bernhard | 1 |
Becker, Mark W. | 1 |
Bindemann, Markus | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 32 |
Reports - Research | 25 |
Reports - Evaluative | 7 |
Dissertations/Theses -… | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 9 |
Postsecondary Education | 5 |
Audience
Location
California | 2 |
Canada | 2 |
Germany | 2 |
United Kingdom | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
Austria | 1 |
Canada (Montreal) | 1 |
Italy | 1 |
Massachusetts | 1 |
Ohio | 1 |
United Kingdom (Glasgow) | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Cantrell, Lisa M.; Kanjlia, Shipra; Harrison, Mirjam; Luck, Steven J.; Oakes, Lisa M. – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Infants' ability to perform visual short-term memory (VSTM) tasks develops rapidly between 6 and 8 months. Here we tested the hypothesis that infants' VSTM performance is influenced by their ability to individuate simultaneously presented objects. We used a "one-shot change detection task" to ask whether 6-month-old infants (N = 47)…
Descriptors: Cues, Infants, Visual Perception, Short Term Memory
Wilson, Amanda H.; Alsius, Agnès; Parè, Martin; Munhall, Kevin G. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2016
Purpose: The aim of this article is to examine the effects of visual image degradation on performance and gaze behavior in audiovisual and visual-only speech perception tasks. Method: We presented vowel-consonant-vowel utterances visually filtered at a range of frequencies in visual-only, audiovisual congruent, and audiovisual incongruent…
Descriptors: Vowels, Phonemes, Experiments, Eye Movements
Jordan, Timothy R.; McGowan, Victoria A.; Kurtev, Stoyan; Paterson, Kevin B. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
When reading from left to right, useful information acquired during each fixational pause is widely assumed to extend 14 to 15 characters to the right of fixation but just 3 to 4 characters to the left, and certainly no further than the beginning of the fixated word. However, this leftward extent is strikingly small and seems inconsistent with…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Reading Processes, Experiments, Visual Discrimination
Gagl, Benjamin; Hawelka, Stefan; Richlan, Fabio; Schuster, Sarah; Hutzler, Florian – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
The study investigated parafoveal preprocessing by the means of the classical invisible boundary paradigm and a novel manipulation of the parafoveal previews (i.e., visual degradation). Eye movements were investigated on 5-letter target words with constraining (i.e., highly informative) initial letters or similarly constraining final letters.…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Word Recognition, Reading Processes, Visual Perception
Strachan, James W. A.; Kirkham, Alexander J.; Manssuer, Luis R.; Tipper, Steven P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Eye gaze is a powerful directional cue that automatically evokes joint attention states. Even when faces are ignored, there is incidental learning of the reliability of the gaze cueing of another person, such that people who look away from targets are judged less trustworthy. In a series of experiments, we demonstrated further properties of the…
Descriptors: Incidental Learning, Trust (Psychology), Psychological Patterns, Visual Perception
Pan, Jinger; Yan, Ming; Laubrock, Jochen; Shu, Hua; Kliegl, Reinhold – Developmental Science, 2013
We measured Chinese dyslexic and control children's eye movements during rapid automatized naming (RAN) with alphanumeric (digits) and symbolic (dice surfaces) stimuli. Both types of stimuli required identical oral responses, controlling for effects associated with speech production. Results showed that naming dice was much slower than naming…
Descriptors: Experiments, Comparative Analysis, Visual Stimuli, Dyslexia
Angele, Bernhard; Rayner, Keith – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
One of the words that readers of English skip most often is the definite article "the". Most accounts of reading assume that in order for a reader to skip a word, it must have received some lexical processing. The definite article is skipped so regularly, however, that the oculomotor system might have learned to skip the letter string…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Sentences, Verbs, Language Processing
Perez, Dorine Vergilino; Lemoine, Christelle; Sieroff, Eric; Ergis, Anne-Marie; Bouhired, Redha; Rigault, Emilie; Dore-Mazars, Karine – Neuropsychologia, 2012
Words presented to the right visual field (RVF) are recognized more readily than those presented to the left visual field (LVF). Whereas the attentional bias theory proposes an explanation in terms of attentional imbalance between visual fields, the attentional advantage theory assumes that words presented to the RVF are processed automatically…
Descriptors: Evidence, Verbal Stimuli, Word Recognition, Visual Perception
Risse, Sarah; Kliegl, Reinhold – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
During reading information is acquired from word(s) beyond the word that is currently looked at. It is still an open question whether such parafoveal information can influence the current viewing of a word, and if so, whether such parafoveal-on-foveal effects are attributable to distributed processing or to mislocated fixations which occur when…
Descriptors: Evidence, Familiarity, Word Frequency, Reading
Hout, Michael C.; Goldinger, Stephen D. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
When observers search for a target object, they incidentally learn the identities and locations of "background" objects in the same display. This learning can facilitate search performance, eliciting faster reaction times for repeated displays. Despite these findings, visual search has been successfully modeled using architectures that maintain no…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Incidental Learning, Search Strategies, Human Body
Fair, Joseph; Flom, Ross; Jones, Jacob; Martin, Justin – Child Development, 2012
Six-month-olds reliably discriminate different monkey and human faces whereas 9-month-olds only discriminate different human faces. It is often falsely assumed that perceptual narrowing reflects a permanent change in perceptual abilities. In 3 experiments, ninety-six 12-month-olds' discrimination of unfamiliar monkey faces was examined. Following…
Descriptors: Primatology, Infants, Human Body, Experiments
Johnson, Cheryl I.; Mayer, Richard E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2012
In three studies, eye movements of participants were recorded while they viewed a single-slide multimedia presentation about how car brakes work. Some of the participants saw an integrated presentation in which each segment of words was presented near its corresponding area of the diagram (integrated group, Experiments 1 and 3) or an integrated…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Literary Genres, Human Body, Scores
Dalvit, Silvia; Eimer, Martin – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Previous research has shown that the detection of a visual target can be guided not only by the temporal integration of two percepts, but also by integrating a percept and an image held in working memory. Behavioral and event-related brain potential (ERP) measures were obtained in a target detection task that required temporal integration of 2…
Descriptors: Intervals, Short Term Memory, Eye Movements, Stimuli
Becker, Stefanie I.; Horstmann, Gernot; Remington, Roger W. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Several different explanations have been proposed to account for the search asymmetry (SA) for angry schematic faces (i.e., the fact that an angry face target among friendly faces can be found faster than vice versa). The present study critically tested the perceptual grouping account, (a) that the SA is not due to emotional factors, but to…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Human Body, Visual Stimuli, Classification
Tsang, Cara; Chambers, Craig G. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
Cantonese shape classifiers encode perceptual information that is characteristic of their associated nouns, although certain nouns are exceptional. For example, the classifier "tiu" occurs primarily with nouns for long-narrow-flexible objects (e.g., scarves, snakes, and ropes) and also occurs with the noun for a (short, rigid) key. In 3…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Comprehension, Semantics, Nouns