Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Figurative Language | 9 |
Language Processing | 5 |
Children | 3 |
Semantics | 3 |
Word Recognition | 3 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Classification | 2 |
Decision Making | 2 |
Eye Movements | 2 |
Foreign Countries | 2 |
Models | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Discourse Processes: A… | 3 |
Journal of Autism and… | 1 |
Journal of Child Language | 1 |
Journal of Cognition and… | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Journal of Memory and Language | 1 |
Language and Cognitive… | 1 |
Author
Pexman, Penny M. | 9 |
Climie, Emma A. | 2 |
Glenwright, Melanie | 2 |
Hino, Yasushi | 2 |
Lupker, Stephen J. | 2 |
Whalen, Juanita M. | 2 |
Glenwright, Melanie R. | 1 |
Green, Jill J. | 1 |
James, Tammy | 1 |
Krol, Andrea | 1 |
McMorris, Carly A. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 5 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Canada | 2 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Pexman, Penny M.; Rostad, Kristin R.; McMorris, Carly A.; Climie, Emma A.; Stowkowy, Jacqueline; Glenwright, Melanie R. – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2011
We examined processing of verbal irony in three groups of children: (1) 18 children with high-functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder (HFASD), (2) 18 typically-developing children, matched to the first group for verbal ability, and (3) 18 typically-developing children matched to the first group for chronological age. We utilized an irony…
Descriptors: Age, Autism, Figurative Language, Discourse Analysis
Pexman, Penny M.; Whalen, Juanita M.; Green, Jill J. – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2010
Recently, irony researchers have emphasized that irony interpretation involves metarepresentational inferencing in order that the perceiver can determine whether the speaker's attitude is counterfactual to their statement. This research investigated whether the perception of irony also depends on the extent to which an ironic statement is suitably…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Figurative Language, Sociolinguistics, Interpersonal Communication
Glenwright, Melanie; Pexman, Penny M. – Journal of Child Language, 2010
Adults distinguish between ironic remarks directed at targets (sarcasm) and ironic remarks not directed at specific targets. We investigated the development of children's appreciation for this distinction by presenting these speech acts to 71 five- to six-year-olds and 71 nine- to ten-year-olds. Five- to six-year-olds were beginning to understand…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Speech Acts, Negative Attitudes, Figurative Language
Whalen, Juanita M.; Pexman, Penny M. – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2010
A number of studies have now examined the development of children's appreciation for verbal irony, typically by testing children's comprehension of the ironic speaker's belief and intent. This article examines a topic that has received much less attention: children's ability to produce irony in context-appropriate ways. The study presents 7- to…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Verbal Communication, Synchronous Communication, Children
Climie, Emma A.; Pexman, Penny M. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2008
We investigated how children solve the interpretive problem of verbal irony. Children 5 to 8 years of age and a group of adults were presented with ironic and literal remarks in the context of short puppet shows. The speaker puppet's personality was manipulated as a cue to intent; that is, speakers were described as funny or serious. We measured…
Descriptors: Puppetry, Nonverbal Communication, Figurative Language, Human Body
Siakaluk, Paul D.; Pexman, Penny M.; Sears, Christopher R.; Owen, William J. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2007
The ambiguity disadvantage (slower processing of ambiguous words relative to unambiguous words) has been taken as evidence for a distributed semantic representational system like that embodied in parallel distributed processing (PDP) models. In the present study, we investigated whether semantic ambiguity slows meaning activation, as PDP models…
Descriptors: Semantics, Figurative Language, Language Processing, Semiotics
Hino, Yasushi; Pexman, Penny M.; Lupker, Stephen J. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2006
According to parallel distributed processing (PDP) models of visual word recognition, the speed of semantic coding is modulated by the nature of the orthographic-to-semantic mappings. Consistent with this idea, an ambiguity disadvantage and a relatedness-of-meaning (ROM) advantage have been reported in some word recognition tasks in which semantic…
Descriptors: Semantics, Language Processing, Word Recognition, Classification
Pexman, Penny M.; Hino, Yasushi; Lupker, Stephen J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2004
An ambiguity disadvantage (slower responses for ambiguous words, e.g., bank, than for unambiguous words) has been reported in semantic tasks (L. R. Gottlob, S. D. Goldinger, G. O. Stone, & G. C. Van Orden, 1999; Y. Hino, S. J. Lupker, & P. M. Pexman, 2002; C. D. Piercey & S. Joordens, 2000) and has been attributed to the meaning activation…
Descriptors: Semantics, Word Recognition, Figurative Language, Decision Making
Pexman, Penny M.; Glenwright, Melanie; Krol, Andrea; James, Tammy – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2005
Around 5 or 6 years of age, children begin to recognize that speakers who make ironic remarks do not believe what they literally say, but children of the same age do not show appreciation for the humor function of irony (Dews et al., 1996; Harris & Pexman, 2003). We investigated 7- to 10-year-old children's interpretations of verbal irony and…
Descriptors: Humor, Figurative Language, Child Psychology, Psychological Studies