Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 78 |
Descriptor
Food | 100 |
Animals | 91 |
Reinforcement | 76 |
Behavioral Science Research | 64 |
Animal Behavior | 62 |
Responses | 36 |
Intervals | 28 |
Stimuli | 23 |
Color | 22 |
Delay of Gratification | 22 |
Conditioning | 20 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of the Experimental… | 100 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 100 |
Reports - Research | 95 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Adult Education | 2 |
Audience
Location
Florida | 1 |
Indiana | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Expressive One Word Picture… | 1 |
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 1 |
Vineland Adaptive Behavior… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Smethells, John R.; Fox, Andrew T.; Andrews, Jennifer J.; Reilly, Mark P. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
Three experiments investigated the effects of immediate and delayed postsession feeding on progressive-ratio and variable-interval schedule performance in rats. During Experiments 1 and 2, immediate postsession feeding decreased the breakpoint, or largest completed ratio, under progressive-ratio schedules. Experiment 3 was conducted to extend the…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Food, Animals, Operant Conditioning
Cassidy, Rachel N.; Dallery, Jesse – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
The exponential demand equation proposed by Hursh and Silberberg (2008) provides an estimate of the essential value of a good as a function of price. The model predicts that essential value should remain constant across changes in the magnitude of a reinforcer, but may change as a function of motivational operations. In Experiment 1, rats' demand…
Descriptors: Animals, Reinforcement, Stimulants, Food
Lagorio, Carla H.; Hackenberg, Timothy D. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
Pigeons were given repeated choices between variable and fixed numbers of token reinforcers (stimulus lamps arrayed above the response keys), with each earned token exchangeable for food. The number of tokens provided by the fixed-amount option remained constant within blocks of sessions, but varied parametrically across phases, assuming values of…
Descriptors: Token Economy, Positive Reinforcement, Animal Behavior, Behavioral Science Research
Osugi, Mizuho; Foster T. Mary; Temple, William; Poling, Alan – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
Brushtail possums ("Trichosurus vulpecula") were trained to press a right lever when a tone was presented (a tone-on trial) and a left lever when a tone was not presented (a tone-off trial) to gain access to food. During training the tone was set at 80 dB(A), with a frequency of 0.88 kH for 3 possums and of 4 kH for the other 2. Once accuracy was…
Descriptors: Animals, Training, Auditory Stimuli, Food
Feuerbacher, Erica; Wynne, Clive D. L. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
Despite the intimate relationship dogs share with humans in Western society, we know relatively little about the variables that produce and maintain dog social behavior towards humans. One possibility is that human social interaction is itself a reinforcer for dog behavior. As an initial assessment of the variables that might maintain dog social…
Descriptors: Animals, Animal Behavior, Interaction, Reinforcement
Jimenez-Gomez, Corina; Shahan, Timothy A. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
An extensive body of research using concurrent-chains schedules of reinforcement has shown that choice for one of two differentially valued food-associated stimuli is dependent upon the overall temporal context in which those stimuli are embedded. The present experiments examined whether the concurrent chains procedure was useful for the study of…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Reinforcement, Intervals, Food
Boutros, Nathalie; Davison, Michael; Elliffe, Douglas – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
Conditioned reinforcer effects may be due to the stimulus' discriminative rather than its strengthening properties. While this was demonstrated in a frequently-changing choice procedure, a single attempt to replicate in a relatively static choice environment failed. We contend that this was because the information provided by the stimuli was…
Descriptors: Conditioning, Stimuli, Reinforcement, Animals
Boutros, Nathalie; Elliffe, Douglas; Davison, Michael – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
Reinforcers may increase operant responding via a response-strengthening mechanism whereby the probability of the preceding response increases, or via some discriminative process whereby the response more likely to provide subsequent reinforcement becomes, itself, more likely. We tested these two accounts. Six pigeons responded for food…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Probability, Animals, Evaluation Methods
Mazur, James E. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
Parallel experiments with rats and pigeons examined whether the size of a pre-trial ratio requirement would affect choices in a self-control situation. In different conditions, either 1 response or 40 responses were required before each trial. In the first half of each experiment, an adjusting-ratio schedule was used, in which subjects could…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Animal Behavior, Research, Animals
Cowie, Sarah; Davison, Michael; Elliffe, Douglas – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
It has long been understood that food deliveries may act as signals of future food location, and not only as strengtheners of prefood responding as the law of effect suggests. Recent research has taken this idea further--the main effect of food deliveries, or other "reinforcers", may be signaling rather than strengthening. The present experiment…
Descriptors: Conditioning, Stimuli, Reinforcement, Animals
Belke, Terry W. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
Previous research suggested that allocation of responses on concurrent schedules of wheel-running reinforcement was less sensitive to schedule differences than typically observed with more conventional reinforcers. To assess this possibility, 16 female Long Evans rats were exposed to concurrent FR FR schedules of reinforcement and the schedule…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Animals, Physical Activities, Food
Eppolito, Amy K.; France, Charles P.; Gerak, Lisa R. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
Delay to delivery of a reinforcer can decrease responding for that reinforcer and increase responding for smaller reinforcers that are available concurrently and delivered without delay; acute administration of drugs can alter responding for large, delayed reinforcers, although the impact of chronic treatment on delay discounting is not well…
Descriptors: Animals, Delay of Gratification, Reinforcement, Responses
White, K. Geoffrey; Brown, Glenn S. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
Pigeons performed a delayed matching-to-sample task in which large or small reinforcers for correct remembering were signaled during the retention interval. Accuracy was low when small reinforcers were signaled, and high when large reinforcers were signaled (the signaled magnitude effect). When the reinforcer-size cue was switched from small to…
Descriptors: Animals, Reinforcement, Accuracy, Memory
Campos, Heloisa Cursi; Debert, Paula; Barros, Romariz da Silva; McIlvane, William J. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
A go/no-go procedure with compound stimuli typically establishes emergent behavior that parallels in structure and typical outcome that of conventional tests for symmetric, transitive, and equivalence relations in normally capable adults. The present study employed a go/no-go compound stimulus procedure with pigeons. During training, pecks to…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Mental Retardation, Animals, Animal Behavior
Radu, Peter T.; Yi, Richard; Bickel, Warren K.; Gross, James J.; McClure, Samuel M. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
Rewards that are not immediately available are discounted compared to rewards that are immediately available. The more a person discounts a delayed reward, the more likely that person is to have a range of behavioral problems, including clinical disorders. This latter observation has motivated the search for ions that reduce discounting. One…
Descriptors: Rewards, Intervention, Delay of Gratification, Attention