Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 120 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 208 |
Descriptor
Child Development | 217 |
Foreign Countries | 217 |
Statistical Analysis | 217 |
Questionnaires | 57 |
Preschool Children | 54 |
Age Differences | 51 |
Correlation | 51 |
Comparative Analysis | 48 |
Children | 41 |
Longitudinal Studies | 41 |
Gender Differences | 38 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Anders, Yvonne | 2 |
Carlo, Waldemar A. | 2 |
Chomba, Elwyn | 2 |
Fernald, Lia C. H. | 2 |
Kagawa, Rose M. C. | 2 |
Kluczniok, Katharina | 2 |
Knauer, Heather A. | 2 |
Lehrl, Simone | 2 |
Lerkkanen, Marja-Kristiina | 2 |
Li, Hui | 2 |
Lin, Yen-Chun | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 210 |
Reports - Research | 203 |
Reports - Evaluative | 8 |
Information Analyses | 4 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 4 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 2 |
Books | 1 |
Collected Works - General | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 53 |
Elementary Education | 37 |
Preschool Education | 28 |
Primary Education | 25 |
Kindergarten | 19 |
Intermediate Grades | 10 |
Middle Schools | 10 |
Secondary Education | 9 |
Grade 1 | 8 |
Adult Education | 7 |
Grade 2 | 7 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Practitioners | 1 |
Location
Australia | 20 |
Germany | 19 |
United Kingdom | 15 |
Turkey | 13 |
Canada | 11 |
Italy | 11 |
China | 10 |
Netherlands | 10 |
Finland | 6 |
Sweden | 6 |
United Kingdom (England) | 6 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
United Nations Convention on… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Martin Brunner; Sophie E. Stallasch; Cordula Artelt; Oliver Lüdtke – Educational Psychology Review, 2025
There is a need for robust evidence about which educational interventions work in preschool to foster children's cognitive and socio-emotional learning (SEL) outcomes. Lab-based individually randomized experiments can develop and refine such interventions, and field-based randomized experiments (e.g., cluster randomized trials) evaluate their…
Descriptors: Preschools, Social Emotional Learning, Outcomes of Education, Cognitive Objectives
Lindow, Stefanie; Betsch, Tilmann – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2018
In many decision situations, individuals must actively search information before they can make a satisfying choice. In such instances, individuals must be aware of the fact that not all information may be equally relevant for the choice at hand--thus, individuals should weight information by its respective relevance. We compared children's and…
Descriptors: Decision Making, Children, Information Seeking, Adults
Li, Wei; Cao, Bihua; Hu, Lijuan; Li, Fuhong – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2017
Children younger than three years old are able to detect hidden rules in numerical sequences, and this ability matches that of adults by age seven. However, the developmental trajectory of this ability during the ages of four to six remains unknown. The present study adopted a modified Brixton task to address this issue. In this task, children…
Descriptors: Child Development, Age Differences, Numbers, Preschool Children
Shastina, Elena; Shatunova, Olga; Borodina, Tatyana; Borisov, Anatoly; Maliy, Yury – Journal of Social Studies Education Research, 2018
The article explores the cultural potential of reading as one of the factors in the development of children's giftedness. The role of the book as the bearer of culture in the process of preserving national identity is revealed. The authors of the article discover the growing importance of family reading in the process of a gifted person…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Reading, Cultural Influences, Academically Gifted
Peterson, Candida C.; Slaughter, Virginia; Wellman, Henry M. – Developmental Psychology, 2018
Persuasion is an essential social skill. Yet its development and underpinnings are poorly understood. In 2 studies, a total of 167 children aged 3 to 12 years took theory of mind (ToM) tests and participated in unscripted, seminaturalistic persuasive conversations. Children were typically developing (TD) or had deafness or autism spectrum disorder…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Persuasive Discourse, Interpersonal Competence, Children
Lubke, Gitta H.; McArtor, Daniel B.; Boomsma, Dorret I.; Bartels, Meike – Developmental Psychology, 2018
Longitudinal data from a large sample of twins participating in the Netherlands Twin Register (n = 42,827, age range 3-16) were analyzed to investigate the genetic and environmental contributions to childhood aggression. Genetic auto-regressive (simplex) models were used to assess whether the same genes are involved or whether new genes come into…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Longitudinal Studies, Twins, Aggression
Garvis, Susanne; Phillipson, Sivanes; Phillipson, Shane N. – Review of Research in Education, 2021
Early childhood education and care (ECEC) remains a priority area for public policy, internationally and in Australia. However, an analysis of empirical research published internationally up to 2008 has identified a bias toward positivist methodologies within a "scientific/psychological' rather than educational perspective and with a focus on…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Early Childhood Education, Educational Research, Research Methodology
Fletcher, Fay E.; Foster-Owens, Mistral D.; Conduit, Russell; Rinehart, Nicole J.; Riby, Deborah M.; Cornish, Kim M. – Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 2017
The present study compared the course of parent-report and actigraphy-derived sleep profiles over a 1-year period, in school-age children with autism spectrum disorder and typically developing children. The Children's Sleep Habits Questionnaire and 14 nights of actigraphy were used to assess sleep profiles. Parents also completed the Spence…
Descriptors: Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Sleep, Anxiety
Óturai, Gabriella; Kolling, Thorsten; Knopf, Monika – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2018
Findings from previous cross-sectional studies showed that while toddlers around their first birthday imitate selectively, that is, they systematically omit some kinds of target action steps or they copy only the goal, but not the means of the modeled actions, older toddlers imitate more exactly. The aim of the present article is to provide…
Descriptors: Infants, Child Development, Imitation, Individual Differences
Utami, Sri; Nursalam; Hargono, Rachmat; Susilaningrum, Rekawati – International Journal of Evaluation and Research in Education, 2016
The purpose of this study was to analyze the performance of midwives based on the task commitment. This was an observational analytic with cross sectional approach. Multistage random sampling was used to determine the public health center, proportional random sampling to selected participants. The samples were 222 midwives in the public health…
Descriptors: Allied Health Personnel, Obstetrics, Child Development, Identification
Hardecker, Susanne; Schmidt, Marco F. H.; Tomasello, Michael – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2017
Much research has investigated how children relate to norms taught to them by adult authorities. Very few studies have investigated norms that arise out of children's own peer interactions. In two studies, we investigated how 5- and 7-year-old children teach, enforce, and understand rules that they either created themselves or were taught by an…
Descriptors: Child Development, Behavior Standards, Social Behavior, Children
Zachariou, Antonia; Whitebread, David – International Journal of Educational Psychology, 2017
The present study set out to investigate theoretical speculations that regulation and musical play, an initial manifestation of musicality, are directly linked. This study aimed to explore the potential for regulation to occur during musical play and investigate the nature of the regulatory behaviours. Thirty-six children, aged 6 and 8, were…
Descriptors: Music, Play, Statistical Analysis, Elementary School Students
Vasc, Dermina; Miclea, Mircea – Early Child Development and Care, 2018
Iconic gestures illustrate complex meanings and clarify and enrich the speech they accompany. Little is known, however, about how children use iconic gestures in the absence of speech. In this study, we used a cross-sectional design to investigate how 3-, 4- and 5-year-old children (N = 51) communicate using pantomime iconic gestures. Children…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Preschool Children, Nonverbal Communication, Case Studies
Kucirkova, Natalia; Littleton, Karen; Kyparissiadis, Antonios – British Journal of Educational Technology, 2018
This study is the first to systematically investigate the influence of child gender and age, on parents' perceptions of UK children's digital media use at home. It provides an in-depth exploration of how children's age and gender influence the balance between children's use of digital and non-digital media at home. The data draw on 709 parents'…
Descriptors: Gender Differences, Age Differences, Statistical Analysis, Content Analysis
Smith, Lee; van Jaarsveld, Cornelia H. M.; Llewellyn, Clare H.; Fildes, Alison; López Sánchez, Guillermo Felipe; Wardle, Jane; Fisher, Abigail – Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 2017
Purpose: Variability in the timing of infant developmental milestones is poorly understood. We used a twin analysis to estimate genetic and environmental influences on motor development and activity levels in infancy. Method: Data were from the Gemini Study, a twin birth cohort of 2,402 families with twins born in the United Kingdom in 2007.…
Descriptors: Genetics, Environmental Influences, Child Development, Twins