Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 37 |
Descriptor
Form Classes (Languages) | 44 |
Language Processing | 20 |
Nouns | 13 |
Sentences | 13 |
Experiments | 10 |
Eye Movements | 10 |
Semantics | 8 |
Models | 7 |
Grammar | 6 |
Verbs | 6 |
Adults | 5 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Memory and Language | 44 |
Author
Fukumura, Kumiko | 2 |
Hampton, James A. | 2 |
Ito, Kiwako | 2 |
Jonsson, Martin L. | 2 |
Kutas, Marta | 2 |
McElree, Brian | 2 |
Moxey, Linda M. | 2 |
Tanenhaus, Michael K. | 2 |
van Gompel, Roger P. G. | 2 |
Andre, V. | 1 |
Arai, Manabu | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 44 |
Reports - Research | 31 |
Reports - Evaluative | 6 |
Reports - Descriptive | 3 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Roland, Douglas; Mauner, Gail; O'Meara, Carolyn; Yun, Hongoak – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
We investigated the role of discourse context in relative clause processing. We first replicated Reali and Christiansen's (2007a) finding that pronominal object relative clauses are easier to process than analogous subject relative clauses (an effect which stands in contrast to previous research on pronominal relative clauses). We then analyzed…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Grammar, Nouns, Expectation
van Gompel, Roger P. G.; Arai, Manabu; Pearson, Jamie – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Three structural priming experiments investigated how monotransitive and intransitive structures are represented. Experiment 1 showed that priming from intransitives was stronger when the verb was the same in prime and target than when it was different, but monotransitive priming was unaffected by verb repetition. We argue that the activation of…
Descriptors: Priming, Verbs, Experiments, Repetition
Patson, Nikole D.; Warren, Tessa – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
There has been considerable psycholinguistic investigation into the conditions that allow separately introduced individuals to be joined into a plural set and represented as a complex reference object (e.g., Eschenbach et al., 1989; Garrod & Sanford, 1982; Koh & Clifton, 2002; Koh et al., 2008; Moxey, Sanford, Sturt, & Morrow, 2004; Sanford &…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Language Processing, Morphemes, Form Classes (Languages)
Wolter, Lynsey; Gorman, Kristen Skovbroten; Tanenhaus, Michael K. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
Listeners expect that a definite noun phrase with a pre-nominal scalar adjective (e.g., "the big"...) will refer to an entity that is part of a set of objects contrasting on the scalar dimension, e.g., size (Sedivy, Tanenhaus, Chambers, & Carlson, 1999). Two visual world experiments demonstrate that uttering a referring expression with a…
Descriptors: Nouns, Linguistics, Language Processing, Models
Hampton, James A.; Passanisi, Alessia; Jonsson, Martin L. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
The modifier effect is the reduction in perceived likelihood of a generic property sentence, when the head noun is modified. We investigated the prediction that the modifier effect would be stronger for mutable than for central properties, without finding evidence for this predicted interaction over the course of five experiments. However…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Interaction, Experiments, Nouns
Cozijn, Reinier; Commandeur, Edwin; Vonk, Wietske; Noordman, Leo G. M. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
Several theoretical accounts have been proposed with respect to the issue how quickly the implicit causality verb bias affects the understanding of sentences such as "John beat Pete at the tennis match, because he had played very well". They can be considered as instances of two viewpoints: the focusing and the integration account. The focusing…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Language Processing, Verbs, Sentences
Szewczyk, Jakub M.; Schriefers, Herbert – Journal of Memory and Language, 2013
Recently, several ERP studies have shown that the human language comprehension system anticipates words that are highly likely continuations of a given text. However, it remains an open issue whether the language comprehension system can also make predictions that go beyond a specific word. Here, we address the question of whether readers predict…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Language Processing, Prediction, Literary Genres
Martin, Andrea E.; McElree, Brian – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
Language comprehension requires recovering meaning from linguistic form, even when the mapping between the two is indirect. A canonical example is ellipsis, the omission of information that is subsequently understood without being overtly pronounced. Comprehension of ellipsis requires retrieval of an antecedent from memory, without prior…
Descriptors: Sentences, Comprehension, Form Classes (Languages), Memory
Fukumura, Kumiko; van Gompel, Roger P. G.; Harley, Trevor; Pickering, Martin J. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
We tested a cue-based retrieval model that predicts how similarity between discourse entities influences the speaker's choice of referring expressions. In Experiment 1, speakers produced fewer pronouns (relative to repeated noun phrases) when the competitor was in the same situation as the referent (both on a horse) rather than in a different…
Descriptors: Nouns, Grammar, Figurative Language, Models
Breen, Mara; Clifton, Charles, Jr. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
This paper presents findings from two eye-tracking studies designed to investigate the role of metrical prosody in silent reading. In Experiment 1, participants read stress-alternating noun-verb or noun-adjective homographs (e.g. "PREsent", "preSENT") embedded in limericks, such that the lexical stress of the homograph, as determined by context,…
Descriptors: Silent Reading, Nouns, Eye Movements, Intonation
Gagne, Christina L.; Spalding, Thomas L. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
Past research has found that the judged likelihood of properties of modified nouns (baby ducks have webbed feet) is reduced relative to unmodified nouns (ducks have webbed feet). Experiments 1-3 replicate the modification effect and demonstrate that this effect is obtained when participants make dichotomous decisions about the truth of such…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Inferences, Concept Mapping, Nouns
Ito, Kiwako; Jincho, Nobuyuki; Minai, Utako; Yamane, Naoto; Mazuka, Reiko – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Two eye-tracking experiments tested how pitch prominence on a prenominal adjective affects contrast resolution in Japanese adult and 6-year old listeners. Participants located two animals in succession on displays with multiple colored animals. In Experiment 1, adults' fixations to the contrastive target (pink cat [right arrow] GREEN cat) were…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Animals, Intervals, Gardening
Dragoy, Olga; Stowe, Laurie A.; Bos, Laura S.; Bastiaanse, Roelien – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
Time reference in Indo-European languages is marked on the verb. With tensed verb forms, the speaker can refer to the past (wrote, has written), present (writes, is writing) or future (will write). Reference to the past through verb morphology has been shown to be particularly vulnerable in agrammatic aphasia and both agrammatic and…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Verbs, Language Processing, Indo European Languages
Verde, Michael F. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
Pictures consisted of component halves from either the same photographic scene (SS) or different scenes (DS). SS pictures possessed the emergent property of forming a continuous scene. In Experiment 1, conjunction lures were created by rearranging halves from studied DS pictures into new DS or SS lures. Despite equally familiar components,…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Error Analysis (Language), Metacognition, Context Effect
Fukumura, Kumiko; van Gompel, Roger P.G. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2010
Research has shown that following a sentence fragment such as "John impressed Mary because...," people are most likely to refer to John, whereas following "John admired Mary because...," Mary is the preferred referent. Two written completion experiments investigated whether such semantic biases affect the choice of anaphor (pronouns vs. names).…
Descriptors: Semantics, Verbs, Linguistics, Sentence Structure