Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 2 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 7 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 17 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 58 |
Descriptor
Generalization | 87 |
Infants | 87 |
Language Acquisition | 25 |
Infant Behavior | 19 |
Learning Processes | 12 |
Novelty (Stimulus Dimension) | 11 |
Toddlers | 11 |
Child Development | 10 |
Classification | 10 |
Cognitive Development | 10 |
Cognitive Processes | 10 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Gerken, LouAnn | 4 |
Graham, Susan A. | 3 |
Nelson, Charles A. | 3 |
Poulin-Dubois, Diane | 3 |
Barr, Rachel | 2 |
Bornstein, Marc H. | 2 |
Koulaguina, Elena | 2 |
Rakison, David H. | 2 |
Schwarzer, Gudrun | 2 |
Shi, Rushen | 2 |
Thiessen, Erik D. | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 80 |
Reports - Research | 70 |
Reports - Evaluative | 8 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 5 |
Opinion Papers | 2 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Dissertations/Theses -… | 1 |
Guides - Non-Classroom | 1 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 4 |
Audience
Practitioners | 1 |
Researchers | 1 |
Location
Australia | 1 |
Canada (Ottawa) | 1 |
China | 1 |
Israel | 1 |
Spain (Barcelona) | 1 |
Tanzania | 1 |
United Kingdom (Sheffield) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Autism Diagnostic Observation… | 1 |
Bayley Scales of Infant… | 1 |
Mullen Scales of Early… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Erin M. Anderson; Apoorva Shivaram; Susan J. Hespos; Dedre Gentner – Grantee Submission, 2023
The ability to generalize previous knowledge to new contexts is a key aspect of human cognition and relational learning. A well-known learning maxim is that breadth of training predicts "breadth of training predicts breadth of transfer." When examples vary in their surface features, this provides evidence that only the common relational…
Descriptors: Learning Processes, Generalization, Transfer of Training, Familiarity
Ori Ossmy; Danyang Han; Patrick MacAlpine; Justine Hoch; Peter Stone; Karen E. Adolph – Developmental Science, 2024
What is the optimal penalty for errors in infant skill learning? Behavioral analyses indicate that errors are frequent but trivial as infants acquire foundational skills. In learning to walk, for example, falling is commonplace but appears to incur only a negligible penalty. Behavioral data, however, cannot reveal whether a low penalty for falling…
Descriptors: Physical Activities, Robotics, Error Patterns, Infants
Bakopoulou, Milena; Lorenz, Megan G.; Forbes, Samuel H.; Tremlin, Rachel; Bates, Jessica; Samuelson, Larissa K. – Developmental Science, 2023
Words direct visual attention in infants, children, and adults, presumably by activating representations of referents that then direct attention to matching stimuli in the visual scene. Novel, unknown, words have also been shown to direct attention, likely via the activation of more general representations of naming events. To examine the critical…
Descriptors: Vocabulary Development, Attention, Eye Movements, Nouns
Ciobha A. McKeown; Carley E. Smith; Timothy R. Vollmer; Lindsay A. Lloveras; Kerri P. Peters – Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 2024
Teaching an infant manual signs is beneficial as it promotes early communication, improves socialization, and can functionally replace behaviors such as crying and whining. Improving early communication also may reduce the probability of an infant engaging in dangerous behavior, like unsafe climbing. The purpose of this study was to extend…
Descriptors: Teaching Methods, Infants, Help Seeking, Nonverbal Communication
Jeancolas, Laetitia; Rat-Fischer, Lauriane; O'Regan, J. Kevin; Fagard, Jacqueline – Journal of Motor Learning and Development, 2022
Infants start to use a spoon for self-feeding at the end of the first year of life, but usually do not use unfamiliar tools to solve problems before the age of 2 years. We investigated to what extent 18-month-old infants who are familiar with using a spoon for self-feeding are able to generalize this tool-use ability to retrieve a distant object.…
Descriptors: Infants, Problem Solving, Equipment, Generalization
Werchan, Denise M.; Amso, Dima – Developmental Science, 2021
Previous work has shown that infants as young as 8 months of age can use certain features of the environment, such as the shape or color of visual stimuli, as cues to organize simple inputs into hierarchical rule structures, a robust form of reinforcement learning that supports generalization of prior learning to new contexts. However, especially…
Descriptors: Infants, Reinforcement, Bias, Stimuli
Sensoy, Özlem; Culham, Jody C.; Schwarzer, Gudrun – Infant and Child Development, 2021
We investigate when infants exhibit knowledge of the familiar size of well-known objects and whether this knowledge is affected by stimulus format, that is, whether the stimuli are presented as real objects or matched pictures. Infants (130 7- and 12-month-olds) saw everyday objects such as sippy cups and pacifiers in their familiar size and novel…
Descriptors: Infants, Visual Stimuli, Pictorial Stimuli, Familiarity
Liu, Siying; Sun, Renji – Journal of Child Language, 2019
Language is conventional because word meanings are shared among different people. The present study examined Chinese infants' understanding of the language convention that different people should generalize words in the same way. Thirteen-month-old Mandarin-speaking Chinese infants repeatedly viewed a speaker providing a novel label for a target…
Descriptors: Infants, Mandarin Chinese, Generalization, Language Usage
Rakison, David H. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2018
The 4 experiments reported here used the preferential looking and habituation paradigms to examine whether 5-month-olds possess a perceptual template for snakes, sharks, and rodents. It was predicted that if infants possess such a template, then they would attend preferentially to schematic images of these nonhuman animal stimuli relative to…
Descriptors: Infants, Habituation, Eye Movements, Animals
Barr, Rachel; Rusnak, Sylvia N.; Brito, Natalie H.; Nugent, Courtney – Developmental Science, 2020
Bilingual infants from 6- to 24-months of age are more likely to generalize, flexibly reproducing actions on novel objects significantly more often than age-matched monolingual infants are. In the current study, we examine whether the addition of novel verbal labels enhances memory generalization in a perceptually complex imitation task. We…
Descriptors: Infants, Monolingualism, Bilingualism, Comparative Analysis
Krok, Windi C.; Leonard, Laurence B. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2018
Purpose: This study was specifically designed to examine how verb variability and verb overlap in a morphosyntactic priming task affect typically developing children's use and generalization of auxiliary IS. Method: Forty typically developing 2- to 3-year-old native English-speaking children with inconsistent auxiliary IS production were primed…
Descriptors: Verbs, Morphology (Languages), Priming, Task Analysis
Radulescu, Silvia; Wijnen, Frank; Avrutin, Sergey – Language Learning and Development, 2020
From limited evidence, children track the regularities of their language impressively fast and they infer generalized rules that apply to novel instances. This study investigated what drives the inductive leap from memorizing specific items and statistical regularities to extracting abstract rules. We propose an innovative entropy model that…
Descriptors: Linguistic Input, Language Acquisition, Grammar, Learning Processes
Swingley, Daniel – Language Learning and Development, 2019
In learning language, children must discover how to interpret the linguistic significance of phonetic variation. On some accounts, receptive phonology is grounded in perceptual learning of phonetic categories from phonetic distributions drawn over the infant's sample of speech. On other accounts, receptive phonology is instead based on phonetic…
Descriptors: Phonology, Vowels, Phonetics, Indo European Languages
Gerken, LouAnn; Dawson, Colin; Chatila, Razanne; Tenenbaum, Josh – Developmental Science, 2015
Infants have been shown to generalize from a small number of input examples. However, existing studies allow two possible means of generalization. One is via a process of noting similarities shared by several examples. Alternatively, generalization may reflect an implicit desire to explain the input. The latter view suggests that generalization…
Descriptors: Infants, Generalization, Familiarity, Syllables
Koulaguina, Elena; Shi, Rushen – Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 2019
Children begin to learn abstract rules at an early age, in an implicit way, without access to rule descriptions. They rely on specific rule instances that they encounter. However, rule instances often co-occur with rule-inconsistent instances. One kind of inconsistent input, non-application instances, constitutes a learnability problem. For…
Descriptors: Infants, Generalization, Linguistic Input, Grammar