NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Finley, Sara – Journal of Memory and Language, 2011
While the vast majority of linguistic processes apply locally, consonant harmony appears to be an exception. In this phonological process, consonants share the same value of a phonological feature, such as secondary place of articulation. In sibilant harmony, [s] and [esh] ("sh") alternate such that if a word contains the sound [esh], all [s]…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Phonology, Experiments, Phonological Awareness
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Finley, Sara; Badecker, William – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Abstract representations such as subsegmental phonological features play such a vital role in explanations of phonological processes that many assume that these representations play an equally prominent role in the learning process. This assumption is tested in three artificial grammar experiments involving a mini language with morpho-phonological…
Descriptors: Play, Vowels, Phonology, Artificial Languages
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Au, Terry Kit-fong; Oh, Janet S.; Knightly, Leah M.; Jun, Sun-Ah; Romo, Laura F. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Childhood experience with a language seems to help adult learners speak it with a more native-like accent. Can analogous benefits be found beyond phonology? This study focused on adult learners of Spanish who had spoken Spanish as their native language before age 7 and only minimally, if at all, thereafter until they began to re-learn Spanish…
Descriptors: Phonology, Child Language, Native Speakers, Pronunciation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jones, Dylan M.; Hughes, Robert W.; Macken, William J. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2006
Three experiments examined whether the survival of the phonological similarity effect (PSE) under articulatory suppression for auditory but not visual to-be-serially recalled lists is a perceptual effect rather than an effect arising from the action of a bespoke phonological store. Using a list of 5 auditory items, a list length at which the…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Phonology, Grammar, Suffixes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stemberger, Joseph Paul – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
It has been shown that the processing of irregular past-tense forms is affected by phonological factors that are inherent in the relationship of the past-tense forms to other words in the lexicon (rhyming families of irregulars) or to their base forms (vowel dominance effects). This paper addresses more ephemeral phonological effects. In a…
Descriptors: Phonology, Language Processing, Morphemes, Sentences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Taraban, Roman – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
According to "noun-cue" models, arbitrary linguistic categories, like those associated with case and gender systems, are difficult to learn unless members of the target category (i.e., nouns) are marked with phonological or semantic cues that reliably co-occur with grammatical morphemes (e.g., determiners) that exemplify the categories. "Syntactic…
Descriptors: Syntax, Nouns, Cues, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Majerus, Steve; Van der Linden; Martial; Mulder, Ludivine; Meulemans, Thierry; Peters, Frederic – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
The nonword phonotactic frequency effect in verbal short-term memory (STM) is characterized by superior recall for nonwords containing familiar as opposed to less familiar phoneme associations. This effect is supposed to reflect the intervention of phonological long-term memory (LTM) in STM. However the lexical or sublexical nature of this LTM…
Descriptors: Phonology, Long Term Memory, Short Term Memory, Language Processing