Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 14 |
Descriptor
Group Membership | 17 |
Peer Relationship | 7 |
Age Differences | 5 |
Child Development | 5 |
Children | 5 |
Early Adolescents | 4 |
Identification (Psychology) | 4 |
Young Children | 4 |
Adolescents | 3 |
Antisocial Behavior | 3 |
Friendship | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 17 |
Author
Zarbatany, Lynne | 2 |
Bakeman, Roger | 1 |
Barhight, Lydia R. | 1 |
Baron, Andrew Scott | 1 |
Boyko, Lisa | 1 |
Brownlee, John R. | 1 |
Buttelmann, David | 1 |
Cairns, Ed | 1 |
Caldwell, Kathryn | 1 |
Carey, Susan | 1 |
Carpenter, Malinda | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 17 |
Reports - Research | 14 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 2 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Junior High Schools | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Secondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
California | 1 |
Germany | 1 |
United Kingdom (Belfast) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Kogachi, Kara; Graham, Sandra – Child Development, 2020
This study examined the effects of racial/ethnic segregation (i.e., overrepresentation) in academic classes on belonging, fairness, intergroup attitudes, and achievement across middle school (n = 4,361; M[subscript age T1] = 11.33 years), and whether effects depended on numerical minority status in school and race/ethnicity. Latent growth curve…
Descriptors: Minority Group Students, Middle School Students, Racial Segregation, Disproportionate Representation
Gruber, Thibaud; Deschenaux, Amélie; Frick, Aurélien; Clément, Fabrice – Child Development, 2019
Group membership is a strong driver of everyday life in humans, influencing similarity judgments, trust choices, and learning processes. However, its ontogenetic development remains to be understood. This study investigated how group membership, age, sex, and identification with a team influenced 39- to 60-month-old children (N = 94) in a series…
Descriptors: Group Membership, Learning Processes, Age Differences, Gender Differences
Ellis, Wendy; Zarbatany, Lynne; Chen, Xinyin; Kinal, Megan; Boyko, Lisa – Child Development, 2018
Peer group interactional style was examined as a moderator of the relation between peer group school misconduct and group members' school misconduct. Participants were 705 students (M[subscript age] = 11.59 years, SD = 1.37) in 148 peer groups. Children reported on their school misconduct in fall and spring. In the winter, group members were…
Descriptors: Behavior Problems, Task Analysis, Group Membership, Hierarchical Linear Modeling
Howard, Lauren H.; Henderson, Annette M. E.; Carrazza, Cristina; Woodward, Amanda L. – Child Development, 2015
Although children can use social categories to intelligently select informants, children's preference for in-group informants has not been consistently demonstrated across age and context. This research clarifies the extent to which children use social categories to guide learning by presenting participants with a live or video-recorded…
Descriptors: Infants, Young Children, Imitation, Group Membership
Buttelmann, David; Zmyj, Norbert; Daum, Moritz; Carpenter, Malinda – Child Development, 2013
Recent research has shown that infants are more likely to engage with in-group over out-group members. However, it is not known whether infants' learning is influenced by a model's group membership. This study investigated whether 14-month-olds ("N" = 66) selectively imitate and adopt the preferences of in-group versus out-group members.…
Descriptors: Infants, Imitation, Preferences, Infant Behavior
Master, Allison; Walton, Gregory M. – Child Development, 2013
Three experiments ("N" = 130) used a minimal group manipulation to show that just perceived membership in a social group boosts young children's motivation for and learning from group-relevant tasks. In Experiment 1, 4-year-old children assigned to a minimal "puzzles group" persisted longer on a challenging puzzle than children identified as the…
Descriptors: Group Membership, Preschool Children, Motivation, Group Activities
The Protective Role of Group Identity: Sectarian Antisocial Behavior and Adolescent Emotion Problems
Merrilees, Christine E.; Taylor, Laura K.; Goeke-Morey, Marcie C.; Shirlow, Peter; Cummings, E. Mark; Cairns, Ed – Child Development, 2014
The protective role of strength of group identity was examined for youth in a context of protracted political conflict. Participants included 814 adolescents (M[subscript age] = 13.61, SD = 1.99 at Time 1) participating in a longitudinal study in Belfast, Northern Ireland. Utilizing hierarchical linear modeling, the results show that the effect of…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Antisocial Behavior, Foreign Countries, Group Membership
Barhight, Lydia R.; Hubbard, Julie A.; Hyde, Christopher T. – Child Development, 2013
Study goals were to explore whether children clustered into groups based on reactions to witnessing bullying and to examine whether these reactions predicted bullying intervention. Seventy-nine children ("M" = 10.80 years) watched bullying videos in the laboratory while their heart rate (HR) was measured, and they self-reported on…
Descriptors: Bullying, Child Development, Emotional Response, Prediction
Weller, Drika; Lagattuta, Kristin Hansen – Child Development, 2013
Five- to 13-year-old European American children ("N" = 76) predicted characters' decisions, emotions, and obligations in prosocial moral dilemmas. Across age, children judged that characters would feel more positive emotions helping an unfamiliar child from the racial in-group versus out-group (African American), happier ignoring the…
Descriptors: Emotional Response, Whites, Prosocial Behavior, Moral Values
Killen, Melanie; Mulvey, Kelly Lynn; Hitti, Aline – Child Development, 2013
"Interpersonal" rejection and "intergroup" exclusion in childhood reflect different, but complementary, aspects of child development. Interpersonal rejection focuses on individual differences in personality traits, such as wariness and being fearful, to explain bully-victim relationships. In contrast, intergroup exclusion focuses on how in-group…
Descriptors: Rejection (Psychology), Social Isolation, Child Development, Interpersonal Relationship
Dunham, Yarrow; Baron, Andrew Scott; Carey, Susan – Child Development, 2011
Three experiments (total N = 140) tested the hypothesis that 5-year-old children's membership in randomly assigned "minimal" groups would be sufficient to induce intergroup bias. Children were randomly assigned to groups and engaged in tasks involving judgments of unfamiliar in-group or out-group children. Despite an absence of information…
Descriptors: Student Attitudes, Resource Allocation, Hypothesis Testing, Young Children
Trentacosta, Christopher J.; Criss, Michael M.; Shaw, Daniel S.; Lacourse, Eric; Hyde, Luke W.; Dishion, Thomas J. – Child Development, 2011
This study investigated the development of mother-son relationship quality from ages 5 to 15 in a sample of 265 low-income families. Nonparametric random effects modeling was utilized to uncover distinct and homogeneous developmental trajectories of conflict and warmth; antecedents and outcomes of the trajectory groups also were examined. Four…
Descriptors: Group Membership, Socialization, Low Income, Mothers
Kalish, Charles W.; Lawson, Christopher A. – Child Development, 2008
Three experiments explored the significance of deontic properties (involving rights and obligations) in representations of social categories. Preschool-aged children (M = 4.8), young school-aged children (M = 8.2), and adults judged the centrality of behavioral, psychological, and deontic properties for both familiar (Experiments 1 and 2, Ns = 50…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Children, Adults, Social Cognition

Bakeman, Roger; Brownlee, John R. – Child Development, 1980
Concludes that the movement from parallel play to group play may be more a matter of minutes that months. For the 41 children observed, who ranged in age from 32 to 42 months, parallel play often functioned in the stream of activities as a bridge to group play. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Group Membership, Peer Relationship, Play, Social Development
Ellis, Wendy E.; Zarbatany, Lynne – Child Development, 2007
Group status was examined as a moderator of peer group socialization of deviant, aggressive, and prosocial behavior. In the fall and 3 months later, preadolescents and early adolescents provided self-reported scores for deviant behavior and group membership, and peer nominations for overt and relational aggression, prosocial behavior, and social…
Descriptors: Peer Groups, Socialization, Prosocial Behavior, Group Membership
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2