Publication Date
In 2025 | 2 |
Since 2024 | 9 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 35 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 90 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 270 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 24 |
Practitioners | 4 |
Teachers | 4 |
Administrators | 1 |
Location
Indonesia | 8 |
Canada | 6 |
Germany | 6 |
Netherlands | 5 |
Australia | 3 |
California | 3 |
China | 3 |
Japan | 3 |
United Kingdom | 3 |
Belgium | 2 |
Canada (Montreal) | 2 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
MacArthur Communicative… | 4 |
Bayley Scales of Infant… | 3 |
Strengths and Difficulties… | 2 |
Center for Epidemiologic… | 1 |
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 1 |
Raven Progressive Matrices | 1 |
Wechsler Intelligence Scale… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Mullins, Christopher W.; Young, Joseph K. – Crime & Delinquency, 2012
Although uniquely positioned to provide insight into the nature and dynamics of terrorism, overall the field of criminology has seen few empirically focused analyses of this form of political violence. This article seeks to add to the understanding of terror through an exploration of how general levels of violence within a given society influence…
Descriptors: War, Homicide, Statistical Data, Law Enforcement
Shultz, Thomas R. – Cognitive Development, 2012
This article reviews a particular computational modeling approach to the study of psychological development--that of constructive neural networks. This approach is applied to a variety of developmental domains and issues, including Piagetian tasks, shift learning, language acquisition, number comparison, habituation of visual attention, concept…
Descriptors: Individual Development, Psychology, Computation, Models
Lennard, John – Visible Language, 2011
This article offers two approaches to the question of "invisible punctuation," theoretical and critical. The first is a taxonomy of modes of punctuational invisibility, identifying "denial, repression, habituation, error" and "absence." Each is briefly discussed and some relations with technologies of reading are considered. The second considers…
Descriptors: Punctuation, Internet, Newspapers, Poetry
Hayden, Angela; Bhatt, Ramesh S.; Kangas, Ashley; Zieber, Nicole – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Part representation is not only critical to object perception but also plays a key role in a number of basic visual cognition functions, such as figure-ground segregation, allocation of attention, and memory for shapes. Yet, virtually nothing is known about the development of part representation. If parts are fundamental components of object shape…
Descriptors: Infants, Visual Perception, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation
Jonkman, Sietse; Everitt, Barry J. – Learning & Memory, 2011
The evidence for a role of the striatum in the acquisition of uncued instrumental responding is ambiguous. It has been shown that post-session infusions of anisomycin into the core of the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) impaired instrumental acquisition, but pre-training lesions of the NAcc suggest that it is not necessary. Recently, we demonstrated that…
Descriptors: Evidence, Inhibition, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Reinforcement
Spangler, Sibylle M.; Freitag, Claudia; Schwarzer, Gudrun; Vierhaus, Marc; Teubert, Manuel; Lamm, Bettina; Kolling, Thorsten; Graf, Frauke; Goertz, Claudia; Fassbender, Ina; Lohaus, Arnold; Knopf, Monika; Keller, Heidi – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2011
The aim of the present study was to investigate whether temperament and cognitive abilities are related to recognition performance of Caucasian and African faces and of a nonfacial stimulus class, Greebles. Seventy Caucasian infants were tested at 3 months with a habituation/dishabituation paradigm and their temperament and cognitive abilities…
Descriptors: Infants, Personality, Habituation, Whites
Larkin, Aoife; Karak, Somdatta; Priya, Rashi; Das, Abhijit; Ayyub, Champakali; Ito, Kei; Rodrigues, Veronica; Ramaswami, Mani – Learning & Memory, 2010
Naive "Drosophila" larvae show vigorous chemotaxis toward many odorants including ethyl acetate (EA). Chemotaxis toward EA is substantially reduced after a 5-min pre-exposure to the odorant and recovers with a half-time of [image omitted]20 min. An analogous behavioral decrement can be induced without odorant-receptor activation through…
Descriptors: Habituation, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Inhibition, Entomology
Ardiel, Evan L.; Rankin, Catharine H. – Learning & Memory, 2010
This article reviews the literature on learning and memory in the soil-dwelling nematode "Caenorhabditis elegans." Paradigms include nonassociative learning, associative learning, and imprinting, as worms have been shown to habituate to mechanical and chemical stimuli, as well as learn the smells, tastes, temperatures, and oxygen levels that…
Descriptors: Learning, Memory, Animals, Literature Reviews
James, David K. – Infant and Child Development, 2010
Learning is defined as a change in behaviour that occurs as a result of experience. It is clear that the fetus can learn by means of habituation, classical conditioning and exposure learning. These types of learning will be discussed in relation to learning in the womb and the memory of learned material after birth. Furthermore, the potential…
Descriptors: Classical Conditioning, Prenatal Influences, Learning Processes, Child Development
Masaki, Takahisa; Nakajima, Sadahiko – Learning and Motivation, 2010
Swimming endows rats with an aversion to a taste solution consumed before swimming. The present study explored whether the experience of swimming before or after the taste-swimming trials interferes with swimming-based taste aversion learning. Experiment 1 demonstrated that a single preexposure to 20 min of swimming was as effective as four or…
Descriptors: Aquatic Sports, Animals, Conditioning, Retention (Psychology)
Bremner, J. Gavin; Slater, Alan M.; Johnson, Scott P.; Mason, Uschi C.; Spring, Jo; Bremner, Maggie E. – Child Development, 2011
From birth, infants detect associations between the locations of static visual objects and sounds they emit, but there is limited evidence regarding their sensitivity to the dynamic equivalent when a sound-emitting object moves. In 4 experiments involving thirty-six 2-month-olds, forty-eight 5-month-olds, and forty-eight 8-month-olds, we…
Descriptors: Infants, Habituation, Early Childhood Education, Young Children
Abdel Razeq, Anwar Ahmad – RELC Journal: A Journal of Language Teaching and Research, 2014
This study investigated the readiness of university students for autonomous learning of English as a foreign language. Data was collected using questionnaires and interviews. The study assessed learners' readiness for autonomous learning across three dimensions: a) learners' perceptions of their educational responsibilities; b) learners' abilities…
Descriptors: Independent Study, English (Second Language), Second Language Learning, Qualitative Research
Gross, Cornelia; Schwarzer, Gudrun – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2010
Three studies were conducted to determine whether 7- and 9-month-old infants generalize face identity to a novel pose of the same face when only internal face sections with and without an emotional expression were presented. In Study 1, 7- and 9-month-old infants were habituated to a full frontal or three-quarter pose of a face with neutral facial…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Infants, Habituation, Generalization
Gaither, Sarah E.; Pauker, Kristin; Johnson, Scott P. – Developmental Science, 2012
We know that early experience plays a crucial role in the development of face processing, but we know little about how infants learn to distinguish faces from different races, especially for non-Caucasian populations. Moreover, it is unknown whether differential processing of different race faces observed in typically studied monoracial infants…
Descriptors: Human Body, Whites, Habituation, Visual Stimuli
Zosh, Jennifer M.; Halberda, Justin; Feigenson, Lisa – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2011
The number of individual items that can be maintained in working memory is limited. One solution to this problem is to store representations of ensembles that contain summary information about large numbers of items (e.g., the approximate number or cumulative area of a group of many items). Here we explored the developmental origins of ensemble…
Descriptors: Employment Projections, Infants, Scientific Concepts, Short Term Memory