NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 12 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Souza, Karina Ap F. D.; Porto, Paulo Alves – Science & Education, 2012
Assuming that textbooks give literary expression to cultural and ideological values of a nation or group, we propose the analysis of chemistry textbooks used in Brazilian universities throughout the twentieth century. We analyzed iconographic and textual aspects of 31 textbooks which had significant diffusion in the context of Brazilian…
Descriptors: Textbooks, Physics, Chemistry, Science Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Haskell, Todd R.; Mansfield, Cade D.; Brewer, Katherine M. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2011
Several psycholinguistic theories have appealed to the linguistic notion of markedness to help explain asymmetrical patterns of behavioural data. We suggest that this sort of markedness is best thought of as a derived rather than a primitive notion, emerging when the distributional properties of linguistic categories interact with general-purpose…
Descriptors: Linguistics, Vocabulary Development, Classification, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
de la Rosa, Stephan; Choudhery, Rabia N.; Chatziastros, Astros – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Recent evidence suggests that the recognition of an object's presence and its explicit recognition are temporally closely related. Here we re-examined the time course (using a fine and a coarse temporal resolution) and the sensitivity of three possible component processes of visual object recognition. In particular, participants saw briefly…
Descriptors: Evidence, Recognition (Psychology), Classification, Sampling
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Decker, Wayne H.; Wheatley, Paula C. – Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1982
One hundred undergraduates learned lists of high- or low-imagery nouns in one column (ungrouped) or in three columns (grouped). Grouped-list recall was significantly greater than ungrouped on the third and fourth trials. Spatial grouping seems to provide important cues which are independent of the words learned or imagery level. (Author/CM)
Descriptors: Classification, Higher Education, Learning Processes, Recall (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Mendelson, Morton J. – Child Development, 1984
Students in grades two, four, six, and college sorted abstract visual patterns that varied both in amount of contour and in type of visual organization (unstructured, simple symmetries, multiple symmetries, and rotational). Results suggested that children attend to both amount of contour and visual organization, but that attention to visual…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, College Students, Elementary Education
Strauss, Mark S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1979
The ability of preverbal infants to abstract a prototypical representation of a category, when presented with examples of an artifically constructed category, was investigated. It was determined that infants could process visual information constructively and could take a more active role in category formation than previously believed. (Author/MH)
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Adults, Classification, Higher Education
Kolbet, Lori L.; Garvey, Jackie – 1987
The ability to allocate attentional resources to relevant aspects of a stimulus event is a critical skill needed for efficient information processing. Evidence suggests that this ability to focus on relevant information without interference is dependent on the nature of the stimulus structure of the information to be processed. To test the…
Descriptors: Attention, Classification, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Style
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Smith, Linda B. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1981
The hypothesis that overall-similarity relations structure both adults' and children's classifications of heterogeneous objects (objects that differ in a variety of ways) was supported in two experiments. When objects varied simultaneously on many dimensions, adults and children constructed classifications that maximized within-category similarity…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Dimensional Preference
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Enns, James T.; Girgus, Joan S. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1985
School-aged children and adults performed tasks examining the relation between selective and integrative aspects of visual attention. In a selective attention task, younger children experienced more interference when elements were closely spaced than did other subjects. In an integrative attention task, age differences were most pronounced when…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Attention, Classification, College Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hayes, Brett K.; Taplin, John E. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1993
Categorization responses of 6 and 11 year olds and adults to test stimuli were examined against predictions derived from 2 models that stressed prototypical features or information about exemplars. For six year olds, only the prototype model fit the data. For the two older groups, both models explained variance in performance. (PAM)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Elementary School Students, Foreign Countries
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Becker, Curtis A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1979
Schuberth and Eimas (EJ 159 939) reported that context and frequency effects added to determine reaction times in a lexical decision (word v nonword) task. The present reexamination shows that context and frequency do interact, with semantic context facilitating the processing of low-frequency words more than high-frequency words. (Author/CP)
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Classification, Context Clues, Higher Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Enns, James T.; King, Katherine A. – Developmental Psychology, 1990
Experiment 1 suggested that age differences in line-drawing interpretation among subjects between 6 and 24 years reflected changes in short-term memory for features and changes in strategies used to integrate features over space and time. Experiment 2 suggested that older observers were more active in their attempts to interpret drawings and that…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Processes, College Students