Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Incidental Learning | 3 |
Responses | 3 |
Language Acquisition | 2 |
Naming | 2 |
Preschool Children | 2 |
Stimuli | 2 |
Child Behavior | 1 |
Developmental Disabilities | 1 |
Imitation | 1 |
Learning Processes | 1 |
Modeling (Psychology) | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Analysis of Verbal Behavior | 3 |
Author
Greer, R. Douglas | 2 |
Fienup, Daniel M. | 1 |
Gilic, Lina | 1 |
Samantha Bergmann | 1 |
Tiffany Kodak | 1 |
Virk, Maninder | 1 |
Yoon, Jessica S. | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 3 |
Reports - Research | 3 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Yoon, Jessica S.; Greer, R. Douglas; Virk, Maninder; Fienup, Daniel M. – Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 2023
Although many neurotypical children acquire untaught word-object relations incidentally from naturally occurring environmental experiences, many children with and without developmental disabilities require specific intervention. This study examined the effects of rotating listener (match and point) and speaker (tact and intraverbal-tact) responses…
Descriptors: Incidental Learning, Naming, Developmental Disabilities, Teaching Methods
Samantha Bergmann; Tiffany Kodak – Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 2024
Parity is one source of automatic reinforcement that increases the probability of verbal behavior that conforms to models provided by the verbal community. Parity as a conditioned reinforcer could explain the acquisition of grammar in the absence of direct, explicit reinforcement. This possibility has been explored in previous research on…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Preschool Education, Verbal Development, Responses
Gilic, Lina; Greer, R. Douglas – Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 2011
Naming is a verbal developmental capability and cusp that allows children to acquire listener and speaker functions without direct instruction (e.g., incidental learning of words for objects). We screened 19 typically developing 2- and 3-year-old children for the presence of Naming for 3-dimensional objects. All 9 3-year-olds had Naming, and 8 of…
Descriptors: Naming, Toddlers, Responses, Stimuli