NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 22 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bion, Ricardo A. H.; Borovsky, Arielle; Fernald, Anne – Cognition, 2013
When hearing a novel name, children tend to select a novel object rather than a familiar one, a bias known as disambiguation. Using online processing measures with 18-, 24-, and 30-month-olds, we investigate how the development of this bias relates to word learning. Children's proportion of looking time to a novel object after hearing a novel name…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Novels, Vocabulary Development, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Benitez, Viridiana L.; Smith, Linda B. – Cognition, 2012
Expectancy-based localized attention has been shown to promote the formation and retrieval of multisensory memories in adults. Three experiments show that these processes also characterize attention and learning in 16- to 18-month old infants and, moreover, that these processes may play a critical role in supporting early object name learning. The…
Descriptors: Infants, Object Permanence, Prediction, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zmyj, Norbert; Jank, Jana; Schutz-Bosbach, Simone; Daum, Moritz M. – Cognition, 2011
It is well documented that in the first year after birth, infants are able to identify self-performed actions. This ability has been regarded as the basis of conscious self-perception. However, it is not yet known whether infants are also sensitive to aspects of the self when they cannot control the sensory feedback by means of self-performed…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Infants, Cognitive Ability, Self Concept
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cacchione, Trix; Call, Josep – Cognition, 2010
Recent research suggests that witnessing events of fission (e.g., the splitting of a solid object) impairs human infants', human adults', and non-human primates' object representations. The present studies investigated the reactions of gorillas and orangutans to cohesion violation across different types of fission events implementing a behavioral…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Infants, Primatology, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Nardini, Marko; Atkinson, Janette; Burgess, Neil – Cognition, 2008
In previous studies, children disoriented in small enclosures used room shape, but not wall colors, to find hidden objects. Their reorientation was said to depend solely on a "geometric module" informationally encapsulated with respect to color. We argue that previous studies did not fully evaluate children's use of color owing to a bias in the…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Geometric Concepts, Color, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Senju, Atsushi; Csibra, Gergely; Johnson, Mark H. – Cognition, 2008
In four experiments, we investigated whether 9-month-old infants are sensitive to the relationship between gaze direction and object location and whether this sensitivity depends on the presence of communicative cues like eye contact. Infants observed a face, which repeatedly shifted its eyes either toward, or away from, unpredictably appearing…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Luo, Yuyan; Baillargeon, Renee – Cognition, 2007
The present research examined whether 12.5-month-old infants take into account what objects an agent knows to be present in a scene when interpreting the agent's actions. In two experiments, the infants watched a female human agent repeatedly reach for and grasp object-A as opposed to object-B on an apparatus floor. Object-B was either (1) visible…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Processes, Learning Processes, Object Permanence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mendes, Natacha; Rakoczy, Hannes; Call, Josep – Cognition, 2008
Developmental research suggests that whereas very young infants individuate objects purely on spatiotemporal grounds, from (at latest) around 1 year of age children are capable of individuating objects according to the kind they belong to and the properties they instantiate. As the latter ability has been found to correlate with language, some…
Descriptors: Linguistics, Infants, Primatology, Developmental Stages
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Plunkett, Kim; Hu, Jon-Fan; Cohen, Leslie B. – Cognition, 2008
An extensive body of research claims that labels facilitate categorisation, highlight the commonalities between objects and act as invitations to form categories for young infants before their first birthday. While this may indeed be a reasonable claim, we argue that it is not justified by the experiments described in the research. We report on a…
Descriptors: Cartoons, Infants, Classification, Merchandise Information
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hespos, Susan J.; Baillargeon, Renee – Cognition, 2006
In the present research, 6-month-old infants consistently searched for a tall toy behind a tall as opposed to a short occluder. However, when the same toy was hidden inside a tall or a short container, only older, 7.5-month-old infants searched for the tall toy inside the tall container. These and control results (1) confirm previous…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Ability, Object Permanence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Mitchell, Peter; Taylor, Laura M. – Cognition, 1999
In three shape-constancy experiments, 4- and 7-year olds viewed a circular disc oriented at a slant. All subjects exaggerated circularity of the disc when they knew the object was a circle. Findings suggest that knowledge of reality contaminates judgments of appearance in circle task and this is the same bias that features in realist errors in…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Infants, Learning, Object Permanence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rosander, Kerstin; von Hofsten, Claes – Cognition, 2004
The emerging ability to represent an oscillating moving object over occlusions was studied in 7-21-week-old infants. The object moved at 0.25 Hz and was either occluded at the center of the trajectory (for 0.3 s) or at one turning point (for 0.7 s). Each trial lasted for 20 s. Both eye and head movements were measured. By using two kinds of…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Processes, Object Permanence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Saxe, Rebecca; Tzelnic, Tania; Carey, Susan – Cognition, 2006
Infants know that humans are exempt from some of the principles that govern the motion of inanimate objects: for instance, humans can be caused to move without being struck. In the current study, we report that infants nevertheless do apply some of the same principles to both humans and objects, where appropriate. Five-month-old infants expect…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Ability, Object Permanence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Luo, Yuyan; Baillargeon, Renee; Brueckner, Laura; Munakata, Yuko – Cognition, 2003
This study examined two alternative interpretations of violation-of-expectation findings that young infants can represent hidden objects. Findings indicated that 5-month-olds succeeded in reasoning about the interaction of a visible and a hidden object even though the 2 objects were never simultaneously visible and a 3- or 4-minute delay preceded…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Infant Behavior, Infants, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wang, S.h.; Baillargeon, R.; Paterson, S. – Cognition, 2005
Recent research on infants' responses to occlusion and containment events indicates that, although some violations of the continuity principle are detected at an early age e.g. Aguiar, A., & Baillargeon, R. (1999). 2.5-month-old infants' reasoning about when objects should and should not be occluded. Cognitive Psychology 39, 116-157; Hespos, S.…
Descriptors: Infant Behavior, Object Permanence, Cognitive Psychology, Child Development
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2