Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 10 |
Descriptor
Infants | 20 |
Visual Perception | 20 |
Cognitive Development | 7 |
Cognitive Processes | 7 |
Human Body | 5 |
Adults | 4 |
Age Differences | 4 |
Infant Behavior | 4 |
Perceptual Development | 4 |
Cognitive Ability | 3 |
Motion | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 20 |
Author
Johnson, Scott P. | 2 |
Werker, Janet F. | 2 |
Wilcox, Teresa | 2 |
Wynn, Karen | 2 |
Abbott, Sara | 1 |
Barrett, H. Clark | 1 |
Blass, Elliott M. | 1 |
Bolte, Annette | 1 |
Brannon, Elizabeth M. | 1 |
Camp, Carole Ann | 1 |
Chiang, Wen-Chi | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 20 |
Reports - Research | 19 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 3 |
Audience
Location
Japan | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Frankenhuis, Willem E.; House, Bailey; Barrett, H. Clark; Johnson, Scott P. – Cognition, 2013
Two significant questions in cognitive and developmental science are first, whether objects and events are selected for attention based on their features (featural processing) or the configuration of their features (configural processing), and second, how these modes of processing develop. These questions have been addressed in part with…
Descriptors: Human Body, Infants, Effect Size, Cognitive Processes
Stein, Timo; Sterzer, Philipp; Peelen, Marius V. – Cognition, 2012
The rapid visual detection of other people in our environment is an important first step in social cognition. Here we provide evidence for selective sensitivity of the human visual system to upright depictions of conspecifics. In a series of seven experiments, we assessed the impact of stimulus inversion on the detection of person silhouettes,…
Descriptors: Evidence, Stimuli, Infants, Social Cognition
Mahajan, Neha; Wynn, Karen – Cognition, 2012
A central feature of human psychology is our pervasive tendency to divide the social world into "us" and "them". We prefer to associate with those who are similar to us over those who are different, preferentially allocate resources to similar others, and hold more positive beliefs about similar others. Here we investigate the developmental…
Descriptors: Infants, Interpersonal Attraction, Values, Cultural Influences
Zmyj, Norbert; Jank, Jana; Schutz-Bosbach, Simone; Daum, Moritz M. – Cognition, 2011
It is well documented that in the first year after birth, infants are able to identify self-performed actions. This ability has been regarded as the basis of conscious self-perception. However, it is not yet known whether infants are also sensitive to aspects of the self when they cannot control the sensory feedback by means of self-performed…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Infants, Cognitive Ability, Self Concept
Yoshida, Katherine A.; Iversen, John R.; Patel, Aniruddh D.; Mazuka, Reiko; Nito, Hiromi; Gervain, Judit; Werker, Janet F. – Cognition, 2010
Perceptual grouping has traditionally been thought to be governed by innate, universal principles. However, recent work has found differences in Japanese and English speakers' non-linguistic perceptual grouping, implicating language in non-linguistic perceptual processes (Iversen, Patel, & Ohgushi, 2008). Two experiments test Japanese- and…
Descriptors: Linguistics, Infants, Visual Perception, Japanese
Crookes, Kate; McKone, Elinor – Cognition, 2009
Historically, it was believed the perceptual mechanisms involved in individuating faces developed only very slowly over the course of childhood, and that adult levels of expertise were not reached until well into adolescence. Over the last 10 years, there has been some erosion of this view by demonstrations that all adult-like behavioural…
Descriptors: Maturity (Individuals), Children, Visual Perception, Novels
Bolte, Annette; Goschke, Thomas – Cognition, 2008
Intuition denotes the ability to judge stimulus properties on the basis of information that is activated in memory, but not consciously retrieved. In three experiments we show that participants discriminated better than chance fragmented line drawings depicting meaningful objects (coherent fragments) from fragments consisting of randomly displaced…
Descriptors: Semantics, Infants, Intuition, Semiotics
Yeung, H. Henny; Werker, Janet F. – Cognition, 2009
One of the central themes in the study of language acquisition is the gap between the linguistic knowledge that learners demonstrate, and the apparent inadequacy of linguistic input to support induction of this knowledge. One of the first linguistic abilities in the course of development to exemplify this problem is in speech perception:…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Native Speakers, Infants, Auditory Perception
Gliga, Teodora; Dehaene-Lambertz, Ghislaine – Cognition, 2007
Do infants perceive visual cues as diverse as frontal-view faces, profiles or bodies as being different aspects of the same object, a fellow human? If that is the case, visual exposure to one such cue should facilitate the subsequent processing of the others. To verify this hypothesis, we recorded event-related responses in 4-month-old infants and…
Descriptors: Profiles, Infants, Human Body, Cues
Woods, Rebecca J.; Wilcox, Teresa – Cognition, 2006
Recent research indicates that infants first use form and then surface features as the basis for individuating objects. However, very little is known about the underlying basis for infants' differential sensitivity to form than surface features. The present research assessed infants' sensitivity to luminance differences. Like other surface…
Descriptors: Infants, Visual Stimuli, Visual Perception, Visual Learning

Hespos, Susan J.; Rochat, Philippe – Cognition, 1997
Six experiments assessed 4- to 8-month-old infants' reactions to probable and improbable orientation positions following invisible transformations from an original orientation. Availability of orientation cues, objects' path of motion, and amount of invisible spatial transformation were varied. Results indicated that infants as young as 4 months…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Infants, Motion, Spatial Ability

Jusczyk, Peter W.; Johnson, Scott P.; Kennedy, Lori J.; Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Cognition, 1999
This study compared role of motion in adults' and infants' perception of object unity. Findings favored ecologically-oriented accounts of object perception. Motion was a determinant of object unity for infants. Alignment and common motion contributed to adults' object-unity perception; synchronous color changes did not. Infants detected…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Color, Infants

Wilcox, Teresa – Cognition, 1999
Four experiments examined the perceptual features used by 4.5- to 11.5-month olds to individuate objects involved in occlusion events. Results indicated that 4.5-month olds used shape and size features to individuate objects in occlusion events. By 7.5 months, infants used pattern, and by 11.5 months, they used color to reason about object…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Color, Infants, Pattern Recognition

Chiang, Wen-Chi; Wynn, Karen – Cognition, 2000
Four experiments examined 8-month-olds' ability to reason about collections of objects. Findings suggested that infants' expectations about object behavior do not automatically apply to any and all portions of matter within the visual field. The behavior of an entity and infants' prior experience played roles in determining whether infants will…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Expectation, Infant Behavior, Infants

Gilmore, Rick O.; Johnson, Mark H. – Cognition, 1997
Investigated the nature of spatial representations underlying simple visually guided actions with 3- and 7-month-old infants. Saccades in older infants were executed within body-centered spatial coordinates that account for intervening eye movements, whereas younger infants responded according to the target's retinocentric locations without…
Descriptors: Early Experience, Infants, Perceptual Development, Psychomotor Skills
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2