Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 11 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 45 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 110 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 171 |
Descriptor
Interference (Learning) | 171 |
Foreign Countries | 93 |
Cognitive Processes | 86 |
Visual Stimuli | 72 |
College Students | 68 |
Memory | 62 |
Reaction Time | 61 |
Recall (Psychology) | 55 |
Undergraduate Students | 54 |
Color | 52 |
Short Term Memory | 48 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 171 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 171 |
Reports - Research | 170 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 123 |
Postsecondary Education | 111 |
Audience
Location
Germany | 21 |
Canada | 9 |
North Carolina | 8 |
United Kingdom (England) | 8 |
Israel | 7 |
Netherlands | 5 |
Switzerland | 5 |
Texas | 5 |
Missouri | 4 |
Oregon | 4 |
Pennsylvania | 4 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 33 |
Wechsler Adult Intelligence… | 2 |
Raven Advanced Progressive… | 1 |
Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure… | 1 |
Wechsler Memory Scale | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Caitlin A. Sisk; Vanessa G. Lee – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
Throughout prolonged tasks, visual attention fluctuates temporally in response to the present stimuli, task demands, and changes in available attentional resources. This temporal fluctuation has downstream effects on memory for stimuli presented during the task. Researchers have established that detection of a target (e.g., a square of a color to…
Descriptors: Adults, Memory, Interference (Learning), Recall (Psychology)
Jörg D. Jescheniak; Stefan Wöhner; Herbert Schriefers – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
Adaptive models of word production hold that lexical processing is shaped by recent production episodes. In particular, the models proposed by Howard et al. (2006) and Oppenheim et al. (2010) assume that the connection strength between semantic and lexical representations is updated continuously, on each use of a word. These changes make…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, College Students, Word Recognition, Interference (Learning)
Liang Lin; Xiaowei Ruan; Renjie Liu; Jinli Zhu; Wenhua Zhang; Zhong-Lin Lu; Fan Lu; Fang Hou – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
Perceptual learning (PL) can significantly improve human performance in perceptual tasks primarily through template reweighting. Previous studies have documented how PL changes perceptual template in stimulus feature space. We investigated how PL reweights visual information in time. With a dynamic external noise paradigm and the elaborated…
Descriptors: Perceptual Development, Time Perspective, Visual Learning, Time Factors (Learning)
Nora Turoman; Evie Vergauwe – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
There is growing recognition that working memory and selective attention are highly related. However, a key function of selective attention--ignoring distractors--is much less understood in the domain of working memory. In the attention domain, it is now clear that distractors' task relevance and stimulation of multiple senses at a time (i.e.,…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Undergraduate Students, Short Term Memory, Interference (Learning)
Hurst, Michelle A.; Boyer, Ty W.; Cordes, Sara – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
Although difficulties processing both symbolic and nonsymbolic proportion compared with absolute number are well established, the mechanisms involved remain unclear. We investigate four potential explanations to account for better number processing in adulthood: (a) number is more salient than proportion, (b) number is encoded more automatically…
Descriptors: Attention, Numbers, Cognitive Processes, Interference (Learning)
Dillon H. Murphy; Shawn T. Schwartz; Alan D. Castel – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
Value-directed remembering refers to the tendency to best remember important information at the expense of less valuable information, and this ability may draw on strategic attentional processes. In six experiments, we investigated the role of attention in value-directed remembering by examining memory for important information under conditions of…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Memory, Cognitive Processes, Recall (Psychology)
Hsuan-Fu Chao – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
Repeating a single-prime stimulus as a target to respond to usually facilitates responses. However, sometimes, prime repetition slows the responses and produces the single-prime negative priming effect. In this study, the distractor set hypothesis was proposed as a mechanism of attentional control that can contribute toward single-prime negative…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Priming, Color, Reaction Time
Honami Kobayashi; Hiroshi Matsui; Hirokazu Ogawa – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
Foraging refers to behavior that exploits the current environment for resources and induces exploration for a better environment. Visual foraging tasks have been used to study human behavior during visual searches. Participants searched for target stimuli among the distractors and either acquired or lost points when they clicked on a target or…
Descriptors: Interference (Learning), Information Retrieval, Foreign Countries, Associative Learning
Paul Kelber; Ian Grant Mackenzie; Victor Mittelstädt – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
Context information can guide cognitive control, but both the extent and the underlying processes are poorly understood. Previous studies often found that the congruency sequence effect (CSE) is larger when perceptual context features (e.g., modality and format) of task-related distractors and targets repeat compared to change. However, it is…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, College Students, Cognitive Processes, Learning Modalities
Mulligan, Neil W.; Buchin, Zachary L.; West, John T. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2022
Memory retrieval affects subsequent memory in ways both positive (e.g., the testing effect) and negative (e.g., retrieval-induced forgetting, RIF). The changes to memory that retrieval produces can be thought of as the encoding consequences of retrieval, examined here with respect to attention. In three experiments, participants first studied…
Descriptors: Attention, Testing, Recall (Psychology), Memory
Stefan Wöhner; Andreas Mädebach; Herbert Schriefers; Jörg D. Jescheniak – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
This study traced different types of distractor effects in the picture-word interference (PWI) task across repeated naming. Starting point was a PWI study by Kurtz et al. (2018). It reported that naming a picture (e.g., of a duck) was slowed down by a distractor word phonologically related to an alternative picture name from a different taxonomic…
Descriptors: Naming, Interference (Learning), Foreign Countries, College Students
Mulligan, Neil W.; Spataro, Pietro; Rossi-Arnaud, Clelia; Wall, Avery R. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2022
Stimuli presented with targets during a monitoring task are better remembered than stimuli presented with distractors, a result referred to as the attentional boost effect (ABE). The ABE is consistently found for item memory, but conflicting results have been reported for different assessments of associative memory, with studies of source memory…
Descriptors: Attention, Memory, Associative Learning, Interference (Learning)
Mittelstädt, Victor; Mackenzie, Ian Grant; Koob, Valentin; Janczyk, Markus – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
In the present study, we examined how the relevance of potentially distracting information modulates the interplay of target and distractor processing in conflict tasks. Specifically, we manipulated the degree to which distracting information is relevant for performing the overall task by varying the proportion of trials in which a response to the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Attention Control, Conflict, Task Analysis
Tom Mercer; Anna-Maria Markova – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
While visual working memory has a short lifetime, residual representations can persist and disrupt currently maintained information. This phenomenon is known as proactive interference (PI), and the present study investigated whether the representations underpinning item-specific PI lose details over time. This would be expected if the memories…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Short Term Memory, Interference (Learning), Time Factors (Learning)
Emma C. Holtz; Vanessa G. Lee – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
Increasing evidence has shown that implicit learning shapes visuospatial attention, yet how such learning interacts with top-down, goal-driven attention remains unclear. This study investigated the relationship between task goals and selection history using a location probability learning (LPL) paradigm. We tested whether a top-down spatial cue…
Descriptors: College Students, Spatial Ability, Goal Orientation, Visual Learning