NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 16 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vallesi, Antonino; Lozano, Violeta N.; Correa, Angel – Cognition, 2013
Preparation over time is a ubiquitous capacity which implies decreasing uncertainty about when critical events will occur. This capacity is usually studied with the variable foreperiod paradigm, which consists in the random variation of the time interval (foreperiod) between a warning stimulus and a target. With this paradigm, response time (RT)…
Descriptors: Models, Intervals, Reaction Time, Prediction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mou, Weimin; Spetch, Marcia L. – Cognition, 2013
Five experiments examined the integration and competition between body and context objects in locating an object. Participants briefly viewed a target object in a virtual environment and detected whether the target object was moved or not after a 10 s interval. Experiments 1 and 2 showed that performance when both the observer body and the context…
Descriptors: Virtual Classrooms, Competition, Memory, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Green, Jessica J.; Woldorff, Marty G. – Cognition, 2012
The observation of cueing effects (faster responses for cued than uncued targets) rapidly following centrally-presented arrows has led to the suggestion that arrows trigger rapid automatic shifts of spatial attention. However, these effects have primarily been observed during easy target-detection tasks when both cue and target remain on the…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Intervals, Conflict, Attention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zhang, Hui; Mou, Weimin; McNamara, Timothy P. – Cognition, 2011
Three experiments examined the role of reference directions in spatial updating. Participants briefly viewed an array of five objects. A non-egocentric reference direction was primed by placing a stick under two objects in the array at the time of learning. After a short interval, participants detected which object had been moved at a novel view…
Descriptors: Experiments, Spatial Ability, Intervals, Test Results
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dawson, Colin; Gerken, LouAnn – Cognition, 2011
While many constraints on learning must be relatively experience-independent, past experience provides a rich source of guidance for subsequent learning. Discovering structure in some domain can inform a learner's future hypotheses about that domain. If a general property accounts for particular sub-patterns, a rational learner should not…
Descriptors: Evidence, Intervals, Logical Thinking, Auditory Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ono, Fuminori; Kitazawa, Shigeru – Cognition, 2010
The present study examined the effect of perceived motion-in-depth on temporal interval perception. We required subjects to estimate the length of a short empty interval starting from the offset of a first marker and ending with the onset of a second marker. The size of the markers was manipulated so that the subjects perceived a visual object as…
Descriptors: Intervals, Motion, Visual Perception, Time Perspective
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kolinsky, Regine; Lidji, Pascale; Peretz, Isabelle; Besson, Mireille; Morais, Jose – Cognition, 2009
The aim of this study was to determine if two dimensions of song, the phonological part of lyrics and the melodic part of tunes, are processed in an independent or integrated way. In a series of five experiments, musically untrained participants classified bi-syllabic nonwords sung on two-tone melodic intervals. Their response had to be based on…
Descriptors: Intervals, Vowels, Phonology, Music
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sharot, Tali; Yonelinas, Andrew P. – Cognition, 2008
Emotion has been suggested to slow forgetting via a mechanism that enhances memory consolidation. Here, we investigate whether this time dependent process influences the subjective experience of recollection as well as the ability to retrieve specific contextual details of the study event. To do so we examined recognition for emotional and neutral…
Descriptors: Intervals, Memory, Psychological Patterns, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Engbert, Kai; Wohlschlager, Andreas; Haggard, Patrick – Cognition, 2008
The sense of agency ("I did that") is a basic feature of our subjective experience. Experimental studies usually focus on either its attributional aspects (the "I" of "I did that") or on its motoric aspects (the "did" aspect of "I did that"). Here, we combine both aspects and focus on the subjective experience of the time between action and…
Descriptors: Intervals, Self Concept, Reaction Time, Action Research
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Verhoef, Kim; Roelofs, Ardi; Chwilla, Dorothee J. – Cognition, 2009
How are bilinguals able to switch from one language to another? The prevailing inhibition hypothesis takes larger reaction-time (RT) costs for switching to the first language (L1) than to the second language (L2) as evidence for suppression of the non-target language. Switch cost asymmetries can alternatively be explained by an L1-repeat-benefit,…
Descriptors: Intervals, Inhibition, Brain, Bilingualism
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wang, Qi – Cognition, 2009
Cross-cultural studies have shown that Asians exhibit less accessibility to episodic memories than Euro-Americans. This difference is often attributed to differential cognitive and social influences on memory retention, although there have been no empirical data concerning the underlying mechanism. Three studies were conducted to examine encoding…
Descriptors: Intervals, Cultural Differences, Recall (Psychology), Social Influences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kazanina, Nina; Phillips, Colin – Cognition, 2007
Imperfective or progressive verb morphology makes it possible to use the name of a whole event to refer to an activity that is clearly not a complete instance of that event, leading to what is known as the Imperfective Paradox. For example, a sentence like "John was building a house" does not entail that a house ever got built. The Imperfective…
Descriptors: Verbs, Form Classes (Languages), Intervals, Sentences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McDermott, Josh; Hauser, Marc D. – Cognition, 2007
Human adults generally find fast tempos more arousing than slow tempos, with tempo frequently manipulated in music to alter tension and emotion. We used a previously published method [McDermott, J., & Hauser, M. (2004). Are consonant intervals music to their ears? Spontaneous acoustic preferences in a nonhuman primate. Cognition, 94(2), B11-B21]…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Intervals, Music, Music Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Amso, D.; Johnson, S.P. – Cognition, 2005
We used a spatial negative priming (SNP) paradigm to examine visual selective attention in infants and adults using eye movements as the motor selection measure. In SNP, when a previously ignored location becomes the target to be selected, responses to it are impaired, providing a measure of inhibitory selection. Each trial consisted of a prime…
Descriptors: Models, Intervals, Infants, Eye Movements
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McDermott, Josh; Hauser, Marc – Cognition, 2004
Humans find some sounds more pleasing than others; such preferences may underlie our enjoyment of music. To gain insight into the evolutionary origins of these preferences, we explored whether they are present in other animals. We designed a novel method to measure the spontaneous sound preferences of cotton-top tamarins, a species that has been…
Descriptors: Intervals, Acoustics, Auditory Perception, Primatology
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2