NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 21 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brandone, Amanda C.; Gelman, Susan A. – Cognitive Development, 2013
The goal of the present study was to explore domain differences in young children's expectations about the structure of animal and artifact categories. We examined 5-year-olds' and adults' use of category-referring generic noun phrases (e.g., "Birds fly") about novel animals and artifacts. The same stimuli served as both animals and artifacts;…
Descriptors: Animals, Language Usage, Language Acquisition, Cues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kondrad, Robyn L.; Jaswal, Vikram K. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Errors differ in degree of seriousness. We asked whether preschoolers would use the magnitude of an informant's errors to decide if that informant would be a good source of information later. Four- and 5-year-olds observed two informants incorrectly label familiar objects, but one informant's errors were closer to the correct answer than the…
Descriptors: Semantics, Novels, Language Acquisition, Semiotics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Boloh, Yves; Ibernon, Laure – Cognitive Development, 2010
Grammatical gender is generally considered an early and error-free acquisition in French children. This article first examines how children cope with the gender attribution problem, "i.e.", how they determine the gender of individual nouns. We consider the plausibility and requirements of an account in which tacit phonological assignment rules are…
Descriptors: Nouns, French, Grammar, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Seston, Rebecca; Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; Ma, Weiyi; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy – Cognitive Development, 2009
Can 6- and 8-year-olds (and adults) comprehend common instrument verbs when extended to novel situations? Participants heard eight unusual extensions of common verbs and were asked to paraphrase the verbs' meanings. Half of the verbs used were "specified instrument" verbs that include the name of the instrument used to perform the action (e.g., a…
Descriptors: Verbs, Standardized Tests, Novels, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Morgan, Gary; Herman, Rosalind; Barriere, Isabelle; Woll, Bencie – Cognitive Development, 2008
In the course of language development children must solve arbitrary form-to-meaning mappings, in which semantic components are encoded onto linguistic labels. Because sign languages describe motion and location of entities through iconic movements and placement of the hands in space, child signers may find spatial semantics-to-language mapping…
Descriptors: Sentences, Semantics, Sign Language, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Baumer, Sonja; Ferholt, Beth; Lecusay, Robert – Cognitive Development, 2005
This paper examines the effects of the playworld educational practice on the development of narrative competence in 5- to 7-year-old children. The playworld educational practice is derived from play pedagogy and the theory of narrative learning, both developed and implemented in Scandinavia. The playworld practice consists of joint adult-child…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Teaching Methods, Educational Practices, Play
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hall, D. Geoffrey – Cognitive Development, 1996
Four experiments used a free-naming task to examine four-year olds' and adults' default construals of solids and nonsolids. Found that children named an individual-related word (such as shape) for solid materials, but gave a substance-related name for nonsolids. Results suggest that children conceptualize solids and nonsolids in distinct,…
Descriptors: Adults, Concept Formation, Language Acquisition, Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bialystok, Ellen; Codd, Judith – Cognitive Development, 1997
Used a framework-isolating analysis of knowledge and control of processing components to investigate preschoolers' acquisition of cardinality. Found that cardinality emerges gradually in children between ages three and five. Also, tasks that increase the processing burden of a basic counting problem by adding demands for either analysis or control…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Cognitive Development, Language Acquisition, Metalinguistics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ambridge, Ben; Theakston, Anna L.; Lieven, Elena V. M.; Tomasello, Michael – Cognitive Development, 2006
In many cognitive domains, learning is more effective when exemplars are distributed over a number of sessions than when they are all presented within one session. The present study investigated this "distributed learning effect" with respect to English-speaking children's acquisition of a complex grammatical construction. Forty-eight children…
Descriptors: Syntax, Language Research, Language Acquisition, English
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Braine, Martin D. S.; And Others – Cognitive Development, 1993
Examined thematic and grammatical role categories accessible to preschool children and how access to these categories changes with age. Results of three experiments with children and adults confirmed the psychological reality of certain semantic categories, and provided evidence suggesting a transition in the prominence of semantic relative to…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Early Childhood Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
O'Reilly, Anne Watson; Painter, Kathleen M.; Bornstein, Marc H. – Cognitive Development, 1997
Study 1 explored associations between multiple measures of language and symbolic gesture development across ages 3 and 4; Study 2 measured more finely which aspects of language relate to the symbolic representation of actions with objects, and explored associations between symbolic gesture and general intellectual ability. Results showed…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Intelligence, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Forbes, James N.; Farrar, M. Jeffrey – Cognitive Development, 1993
Study of 3 and 7 year olds and adults examined role that changes in continuity, direction, instrument, and causative agent play in children's and adults' initial assumptions about meaning of novel motion verbs and events. Subjects made similar initial assumptions, but children generalized more conservatively than adults to all change types in…
Descriptors: Adults, Early Childhood Education, Language Acquisition, Semantics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Imai, Mutsumi; And Others – Cognitive Development, 1994
A study with three- and five-year olds contrasted two important proposals regarding children's assumptions about word meanings: the taxonomic assumption proposal and the shape bias proposal. Results suggest that perceptual similarity, particularly shape similarity, is very important in early word meaning but that children gradually shift their…
Descriptors: Classification, Concept Formation, Early Childhood Education, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Johnson, Carla J. – Cognitive Development, 1994
Children ages five, seven, and nine years named objects with multiple names in a neutral context and in a biased context. Children selected names in accord with nonlinguistic constraints, but at the cost of longer naming times. Both name selection success and associated cost were more evident in older children than in younger children. (TJQ)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Language, Context Effect, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Saylor, Megan M.; Troseth, Georgene L. – Cognitive Development, 2006
This research investigates preschoolers' use of desires for word learning. Three-year-old children were shown pairs of novel toys and were asked about their own desire and told about a researcher's desire. For half of the children the researcher liked the same object they did and for the other half the researcher liked a different object. The…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Toys, Vocabulary Development, Student Interests
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2