Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Language Proficiency | 9 |
Toddlers | 9 |
Vocabulary Development | 9 |
Language Acquisition | 4 |
Monolingualism | 4 |
Bilingualism | 3 |
English | 3 |
Preschool Children | 3 |
Child Language | 2 |
Code Switching (Language) | 2 |
Cognitive Ability | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Child Language | 3 |
Child Development | 2 |
Child & Youth Care Forum | 1 |
International Journal of… | 1 |
Journal of Cognition and… | 1 |
Language Acquisition: A… | 1 |
Author
Amy Lieberman | 1 |
Davidson, Denise | 1 |
Feldman, Judith F. | 1 |
Fernald, Anne | 1 |
Goldsmith, Lynn | 1 |
Hoff, Erika | 1 |
Im, Haesung | 1 |
Jankowski, Jeffery J. | 1 |
Jennie Pyers | 1 |
Kim, Sojung | 1 |
Koening, Melissa | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 9 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Florida | 1 |
Netherlands | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 2 |
Early Childhood Longitudinal… | 1 |
Expressive One Word Picture… | 1 |
MacArthur Communicative… | 1 |
Preschool Language Scale | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Lauren Berger; Jennie Pyers; Amy Lieberman; Naomi Caselli – Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 2024
Most deaf children have hearing parents who do not know a sign language at birth and are at risk of limited language input during early childhood. Studying these children as they learn a sign language has revealed that timing of first-language exposure critically shapes language outcomes. But the input deaf children receive in their first language…
Descriptors: Deafness, American Sign Language, Native Language, Language Acquisition
Verhagen, Josje; de Bree, Elise; Unsworth, Sharon – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2020
Although a bilingual advantage has been reported for various measures of cognitive control, most previous studies have looked at a limited range of cognitive control measures. Furthermore, they typically leave unaddressed whether positive effects of bilingualism hold for all bilinguals or whether these are modulated by differences in bilingual…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Language Usage, Language Proficiency, Cognitive Ability
Rose, Susan A.; Feldman, Judith F.; Jankowski, Jeffery J. – Child Development, 2015
This study examined the relation of 3-year core information-processing abilities to lexical growth and development. The core abilities covered four domains--memory, representational competence (cross-modal transfer), processing speed, and attention. Lexical proficiency was assessed at 3 and 13 years with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT)…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Toddlers, Language Proficiency, Vocabulary Development
Kim, Sojung; Im, Haesung; Kwon, Kyong-Ah – Child & Youth Care Forum, 2015
Background: Little empirical research examines the process in which home literacy environment (HLE) in toddlerhood is associated with preschoolers' vocabulary and decoding skills using a large-scale dataset. Objective: The purposes of the current study were to (a) examine the differential effect of HLE in toddlerhood on preschoolers' vocabulary…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Preschool Children, Preschool Education, Family Environment
Ribot, Krystal M.; Hoff, Erika – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2014
Relations between bilingual children's patterns of conversational code-switching (responding to one language with another), the balance of their dual language input, and their expressive and receptive proficiency in two languages were examined in 115 2½-year-old simultaneous Spanish-English bilinguals in the U.S. Children were more likely to…
Descriptors: Vocabulary Development, Code Switching (Language), Expressive Language, English (Second Language)
Fernald, Anne; Marchman, Virginia A. – Child Development, 2012
Using online measures of familiar word recognition in the looking-while-listening procedure, this prospective longitudinal study revealed robust links between processing efficiency and vocabulary growth from 18 to 30 months in children classified as typically developing (n = 46) and as "late talkers" (n = 36) at 18 months. Those late talkers who…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Word Recognition, Language Proficiency, Language Processing
Koening, Melissa; Woodward, Amanda – Journal of Child Language, 2012
The current study examined monolingual English-speaking toddlers' (N=50) ability to learn word-referent links from native speakers of Dutch versus English, and second, whether children generalized or sequestered their extensions when terms were tested by a subsequent speaker of English. Overall, children performed better in the English than in the…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Monolingualism, Vocabulary Development, Native Speakers

Davidson, Denise – Journal of Child Language, 1997
Examined the use of the mutual exclusivity constraint in naming objects among young children monolingual in English or bilingual in English/Urdu or in English/Greek. The study used three tests of the constraint: disambiguation, rejection and restriction. Findings revealed that bilingual children used the constraint to a lesser extent than…
Descriptors: Ambiguity, Bilingualism, Child Language, English

Reznick, J. Steven; Goldsmith, Lynn – Journal of Child Language, 1989
A validation study of five checklists for assessing two-year-olds' word production revealed that the lists produced comparable mean production scores, reflected age differences, and preserved individual differences in total production and in production of linguistic categories such as nouns, verbs, open class items, and closed class items.…
Descriptors: Child Language, Individual Differences, Language Acquisition, Language Processing