Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 5 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Source
Second Language Research | 4 |
Cognition | 1 |
First Language | 1 |
Journal of Child Language | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Journal of Psycholinguistic… | 1 |
Author
Cleland, Alexandra A. | 1 |
Connell, Brenda | 1 |
Degani, Tamar | 1 |
Fernald, Anne | 1 |
Gershkoff-Stowe, Lisa | 1 |
Han, ZhaoHong | 1 |
Jager, Bernadet | 1 |
Katz, Albert | 1 |
Liu, Zehua | 1 |
Mehrani, Mehdi B. | 1 |
Meng, Yaxuan | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 10 |
Journal Articles | 9 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Canada | 1 |
Iran | 1 |
Israel | 1 |
Netherlands | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Shao, Zeshu; van Paridon, Jeroen; Poletiek, Fenna; Meyer, Antje S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
There is mounting evidence that the ease of producing and understanding language depends not only on the frequencies of individual words but also on the frequencies of word combinations. However, in two picture description experiments, Janssen and Barber (2012) found that French and Spanish speakers' speech onset latencies for short phrases…
Descriptors: Phrase Structure, Nouns, Word Frequency, Indo European Languages
Zhang, Juan; Wu, Chenggang; Yuan, Zhen; Meng, Yaxuan – Second Language Research, 2020
Although increasing literature has suggested that emotion-label words (e.g., anger, delight) and emotion-laden words (e.g., thief, bride) were processed differently in native language (L1), there was a lack of neuroimaging evidence showing such differences in second language (L2). The current study compared the cortical responses to emotion-label…
Descriptors: Emotional Response, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Diagnostic Tests, Native Language
Jager, Bernadet; Cleland, Alexandra A. – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2016
It is a robust finding that ambiguous words are recognized faster than unambiguous words. More recent studies (e.g., Rodd et al. in "J Mem Lang" 46:245-266, 2002) now indicate that this "ambiguity advantage" may in reality be a "polysemy advantage": caused by related senses (polysemy) rather than unrelated meanings…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Semantics, Nouns, Verbs
Mehrani, Mehdi B.; Peterson, Carole – First Language, 2017
In the present cross-linguistic study two experiments were conducted to investigate the effects of age and linguistic background on response tendencies of preschoolers toward forced-choice questions. A total of 163 2- to 5-year-old children, including 63 Persian speakers, 57 Kurdish speakers and 43 English speakers, were asked a set of…
Descriptors: Contrastive Linguistics, Language Acquisition, English, Indo European Languages
Norman, Tal; Degani, Tamar; Peleg, Orna – Second Language Research, 2016
The present study examined visual word recognition processes in Hebrew (a Semitic language) among beginning learners whose first language (L1) was either Semitic (Arabic) or Indo-European (e.g. English). To examine if learners, like native Hebrew speakers, exhibit morphological sensitivity to root and word-pattern morphemes, learners made an…
Descriptors: Transfer of Training, Second Language Learning, Word Recognition, Morphemes
Han, ZhaoHong; Liu, Zehua – Second Language Research, 2013
We report on a study of first-exposure learners with different first languages (L1s: English, Japanese) to examine their ability to process input for form and meaning. We used a rich set of tasks to tap respectively into processing, comprehension, imitation, and working memory. We show that there are advantages to having a first language (L1) that…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Linguistic Input, Short Term Memory, Native Language
Mueller, Jutta L. – Second Language Research, 2009
Previous research on event-related potentials (ERPs) on second language processing has revealed a great degree of plasticity in brain mechanisms of adult language learners. Studies with natural and artificial languages show that the N400 as well as the P600 component appear in learners after sufficient training. The present experiment tests if and…
Descriptors: Sentences, Form Classes (Languages), Familiarity, Language Processing

Turner, Nigel; Katz, Albert – 1991
Two studies investigated the processing of familiar and unfamiliar figurative language. Subjects read paragraphs containing figurative sentences (proverbs in study 1 and metaphors in study 2) or literal controls; later subjects were given a cued recall test designed to test their memory for contextually inappropriate meanings (a literal cue for a…
Descriptors: Cues, Familiarity, Figurative Language, Foreign Countries
Thorpe, Kirsten; Fernald, Anne – Cognition, 2006
Three studies investigated how 24-month-olds and adults resolve temporary ambiguity in fluent speech when encountering prenominal adjectives potentially interpretable as nouns. Children were tested in a looking-while-listening procedure to monitor the time course of speech processing. In Experiment 1, the familiar and unfamiliar adjectives…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Adults, Language Processing, Form Classes (Languages)
Gershkoff-Stowe, Lisa; Connell, Brenda; Smith, Linda – Journal of Child Language, 2006
Overgeneralization occurs when a child uses the wrong word to name an object and is often observed in the early stages of word learning. We develop a method to elicit overgeneralizations in the laboratory by priming children to say the names of objects perceptually similar to known and unknown target objects. Experiment 1 examined 18 two-year-old…
Descriptors: Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Language Processing, Language Acquisition, Young Children