Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Familiarity | 2 |
Language Research | 2 |
Syllables | 2 |
Child Care Centers | 1 |
Coding | 1 |
Cues | 1 |
Foreign Countries | 1 |
Infants | 1 |
Language Acquisition | 1 |
Morphemes | 1 |
Morphology (Languages) | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Abreu-Mendoza, Roberto A. | 1 |
Aguado-Servín, Oscar A. | 1 |
Arias-Trejo, Natalia | 1 |
Johnson, Elizabeth K. | 1 |
Seidl, Amanda H. | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Mexico (Mexico City) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Arias-Trejo, Natalia; Abreu-Mendoza, Roberto A.; Aguado-Servín, Oscar A. – First Language, 2014
Infants across cultures need to identify the characteristics of their native languages in order to become competent speakers. The means by which Spanish-speaking children learn to produce number-gender linguistic markers has not been sufficiently investigated. Thirty-eight three-year-olds were tested in Berko-like production tasks, in which they…
Descriptors: Spanish Speaking, Morphology (Languages), Native Language, Familiarity
Johnson, Elizabeth K.; Seidl, Amanda H. – Developmental Science, 2009
English-learning 7.5-month-olds are heavily biased to perceive stressed syllables as word onsets. By 11 months, however, infants begin segmenting non-initially stressed words from speech. Using the same artificial language methodology as Johnson and Jusczyk (2001), we explored the possibility that the emergence of this ability is linked to a…
Descriptors: Language Research, Research Design, Infants, Suprasegmentals