NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
International English…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing all 12 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shang Jiang – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2024
It has been well documented that formulaic language (such as collocations; e.g., "provide information") enjoys a processing advantage over novel language (e.g., "compare information"). In natural language use, however, many formulaic sequences are often inserted with words intervening in between the individual constituents…
Descriptors: Phrase Structure, Language Processing, Psycholinguistics, Orthographic Symbols
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Smith, Shelby L.; Ward, Richard T.; Allen, Laura K.; Wormwood, Jolie B.; Mills, Caitlin – Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2022
In today's society, we are constantly absorbing information via text (e.g., news, social media), much of which may be affectively charged. However, to date, little is known about how the affective framing of the text itself may give rise to various affective experiences "during" reading. We examined how subtle changes to wording…
Descriptors: Reading Processes, Affective Behavior, Correlation, Language Usage
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Matthew W. Lowder; Adrian Zhou; Peter C. Gordon – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2024
"Hospital" can refer to a physical place or more figuratively to the people associated with it. Such place-for-institution metonyms are common in everyday language, but there remain several open questions in the literature regarding how they are processed. The goal of the current eyetracking experiments was to investigate how metonyms…
Descriptors: Semantics, Eye Movements, Ambiguity (Semantics), Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wei, Yipu; Evers-Vermeul, Jacqueline; Sanders, Ted M.; Mak, Willem M. – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2021
Interpreting subjectivity in causal relations takes effort: Subjective, claim-argument relations are read slower than objective, cause-consequence relations. In an eye-tracking-while-reading experiment, we investigated whether connectives and stance markers can play a facilitative role. Sixty-five Chinese participants read sentences expressing a…
Descriptors: Discourse Analysis, Persuasive Discourse, Bias, Form Classes (Languages)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zhang, Jun; Wu, Yan – Second Language Research, 2023
Scalar implicatures involve inferring the use of a less informative term (e.g. some) to mean the negation of a more informative term (e.g. not all). A growing body of recent research on the derivation of scalar implicatures by adult second language (L2) learners shows that while they are successful in acquiring the knowledge of scalar…
Descriptors: Pragmatics, Inferences, Second Language Learning, Second Language Instruction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kim, Hyunwoo; Rah, Yangon – Language Learning, 2019
The constructionist approach holds that an argument structure construction, a conventionalized form-meaning correspondence of a sentence, allows language users to efficiently access sentential information. This study investigated whether increased sensitivity to constructional information would enable second language learners to efficiently fuse…
Descriptors: Role, Korean, Native Language, English (Second Language)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Taheri, Sahar; Rezaie Golandouz, Ghafour – Cogent Education, 2021
This study set out to inspect Laufer and Hulstijn's (2001) motivational-cognitive construct of task-induced involvement. Involvement load hypothesis predicts that tasks with the same involvement loads lead to the same amount of learning. To this end, three output task types which induce equal involvement indices of 3 were employed to examine the…
Descriptors: Retention (Psychology), Vocabulary Development, Second Language Learning, Second Language Instruction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Çokal, Derya; Sturt, Patrick; Ferreira, Fernanda – Discourse Processes: A multidisciplinary journal, 2018
Two experiments explored the hypothesis that anaphors and demonstratives signal different procedural instructions: Whereas the anaphor "it" brings a concrete entity into a reader's focus, the demonstrative "this" directs the focus to a predicate proposition in a discourse representation. The findings from an online eye-tracking…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Eye Movements, Form Classes (Languages), Reading Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Coulter, Linda; Goodluck, Helen – Volta Review, 2015
Self-paced reading was used in three experiments to compare the processing of certain kinds of English sentences by adults with typical hearing and adults with hearing loss. Adults with typical hearing use strategies for processing an input sentence, anticipating the structure, and leading to incorrect analyses. One strategy we examine in this…
Descriptors: Reading Processes, Reading Skills, Sentences, English
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gullifer, Jason W.; Titone, Debra – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
We investigated whether cross-language activation is sensitive to shifting language demands and language experience during first and second language (i.e., L1, L2) reading. Experiment 1 consisted of L1 French-L2 English bilinguals reading in the L2, and Experiment 2 consisted of L1 English-L2 French bilinguals reading in the L1. Both groups read…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Second Language Learning, Native Language, French
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tesan, Graciela; Johnson, Blake W.; Crain, Stephen – Brain and Language, 2012
The word "any" may appear in some sentences, but not in others. For example, "any" is permitted in sentences that contain the word "nobody", as in "Nobody ate any fruit". However, in a minimally different context "any" seems strikingly anomalous: *"Everybody ate any fruit". The aim of the present study was to investigate how the brain responds to…
Descriptors: Sentences, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Anatomy, Language Usage
Neufeld, Gerald G. – 1973
Forty bilingual Ss were tested in four experiments to see if decoding of linguistically mixed texts with one-word substitutions in the other language would take more time than decoding of unilingual material. The overall aim was to further explore the nature of the language switch mechanism as it functions in input. In the first experiment,…
Descriptors: Auditory Stimuli, Bilingual Students, Bilingualism, Code Switching (Language)