Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 14 |
Descriptor
Learning | 36 |
Age Differences | 8 |
Preschool Children | 8 |
Elementary School Students | 7 |
Cognitive Processes | 5 |
Infants | 5 |
Young Children | 5 |
Attention | 4 |
Children | 4 |
Comparative Analysis | 4 |
Recall (Psychology) | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 36 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 20 |
Reports - Research | 18 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 3 |
Preschool Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 4 |
Location
Germany | 1 |
Pennsylvania | 1 |
Wisconsin | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Butler, Lucas P.; Gibbs, Hailey M.; Levush, Karen C. – Child Development, 2020
In learning about the world children must not only make inferences based on minimal evidence, but must deal with conflicting evidence and question those initial inferences when they appear to be wrong. Four experiments (N = 144) found that young children were significantly more likely to revise their initial inferences when conflicting evidence…
Descriptors: Young Children, Cognitive Processes, Evidence, Inferences
Walker, Caren M.; Lombrozo, Tania; Williams, Joseph J.; Rafferty, Anna N.; Gopnik, Alison – Child Development, 2017
Three experiments investigate how self-generated explanation influences children's causal learning. Five-year-olds (N = 114) observed data consistent with two hypotheses and were prompted to explain or to report each observation. In Study 1, when making novel generalizations, explainers were more likely to favor the hypothesis that accounted for…
Descriptors: Young Children, Learning, Influences, Observation
Robert L. Nix; Sukhdeep Gill; Michelle L. Hostetler; Mark E. Feinberg; Lori A. Francis; Cynthia A. Stifter; Cheryl B. McNeil; Sarah M. Kidder; Damon E. Jones; Ye Rang Park; Christina N. Kim; Ashleigh G. Engbretson; Sarah M. Braaten; Vivian L. Tamkin – Child Development, 2024
The Recipe 4 Success preventive intervention targeted multiple factors critical to the health and well-being of toddlers living in poverty. This randomized controlled trial, which was embedded within Early Head Start home visits for 12 weeks, included 242 racially and ethnically diverse families (51% girls; toddler mean age = 2.58 years; data…
Descriptors: Parents, Toddlers, Eating Habits, Health Promotion
Schmerse, Daniel – Child Development, 2020
This study investigates whether children's preschool experiences are associated with later achievement via enhanced learning behaviors using data from a German longitudinal study following children (N = 554) from age 3 in preschool to age 8 in second grade. There were two main findings. First, results suggest that more positive learning behaviors…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Preschool Education, Elementary School Students, Preschool Children
Howard, Lauren H.; Henderson, Annette M. E.; Carrazza, Cristina; Woodward, Amanda L. – Child Development, 2015
Although children can use social categories to intelligently select informants, children's preference for in-group informants has not been consistently demonstrated across age and context. This research clarifies the extent to which children use social categories to guide learning by presenting participants with a live or video-recorded…
Descriptors: Infants, Young Children, Imitation, Group Membership
Burman, Jeremy T.; Green, Christopher D.; Shanker, Stuart – Child Development, 2015
Self-regulation is of interest both to psychologists and to teachers. But what the word means is unclear. To define it precisely, two studies examined the American Psychological Association's system of controlled vocabulary--specifically, the 447 associated terms it presents--and used techniques from the Digital Humanities to identify 88 closely…
Descriptors: Self Control, Definitions, Networks, Maps
Jant, Erin A.; Haden, Catherine A.; Uttal, David H.; Babcock, Elizabeth – Child Development, 2014
The effects of parent-child conversation and object manipulation on children's learning, transfer of knowledge, and memory were examined in two museum exhibits and conversations recorded at home. Seventy-eight children (M[subscript age] = 4.9) and their parents were randomly assigned to receive conversation cards featuring elaborative…
Descriptors: Children, Learning, Transfer of Training, Memory
Fisher, Kelly R.; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Newcombe, Nora; Golinkoff, Roberta M. – Child Development, 2013
Shape knowledge, a key aspect of school readiness, is part of early mathematical learning. Variations in how children are exposed to shapes may affect the pace of their learning and the nature of their shape knowledge. Building on evidence suggesting that child-centered, playful learning programs facilitate learning more than other methods, 4-to…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Geometric Concepts, Play, Learning
Cook, Susan Wagner; Duffy, Ryan G.; Fenn, Kimberly M. – Child Development, 2013
Children who observe gesture while learning mathematics perform better than children who do not, when tested immediately after training. How does observing gesture influence learning over time? Children (n = 184, ages = 7-10) were instructed with a videotaped lesson on mathematical equivalence and tested immediately after training and 24 hr later.…
Descriptors: Children, Transfer of Training, Learning, Mathematics Instruction
Brosseau-Liard, Patricia E.; Birch, Susan A. J. – Child Development, 2011
Previous research has demonstrated that preschoolers can use situation-specific (e.g., visual access) and person-specific (e.g., prior accuracy) cues to infer what others know. The present studies investigated whether 4- and 5-year-olds appreciate the differential informativeness of these types of cues. In Experiment 1 (N = 50), children used…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Cues, Accuracy, Learning
Gentner, Dedre; Anggoro, Florencia K.; Klibanoff, Raquel S. – Child Development, 2011
Learning relational categories--whose membership is defined not by intrinsic properties but by extrinsic relations with other entities--poses a challenge to young children. The current work showed 3-, 4- to 5-, and 6-year-olds pairs of cards exemplifying familiar relations (e.g., a nest and a bird exemplifying "home for") and then tested whether…
Descriptors: Young Children, Learning, Classification, Relationship
Richert, Rebekah A.; Robb, Michael B.; Smith, Erin I. – Child Development, 2011
Television has become a nearly ubiquitous feature in children's cultural landscape. A review of the research into young children's learning from television indicates that the likelihood that children will learn from screen media is influenced by their developing social relationships with on-screen characters, as much as by their developing…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Mass Media Effects, Television, Learning
Legare, Cristine H. – Child Development, 2012
Explaining inconsistency may serve as an important mechanism for driving the process of causal learning. But how might this process generate amended beliefs? One way that explaining inconsistency may promote discovery is by guiding exploratory, hypothesis-testing behavior. In order to investigate this, a study with young children ranging in age…
Descriptors: Evidence, Young Children, Testing, Beliefs
Southgate, Victoria; van Maanen, Catharine; Csibra, Gergely – Child Development, 2007
Tomasello, Carpenter, and Liszkowski (2007) present compelling data to support the view that infant pointing, from the outset, is communicative and deployed in many of the same situations in which adults would ordinarily point for one another, either to share their interest in something, or to informatively help the other person. This commentary…
Descriptors: Infants, Nonverbal Communication, Learning, Motivation

Rose, Susan A.; Futterweit, Lorelle R.; Jankowski, Jeffrey J. – Child Development, 1999
Examined the relation of positive affect to attention and learning in 5-, 7-, and 9-month olds. Found that at all ages positive affect was associated with long look durations and slower learning. Neutral affect was associated with short looks and faster learning. Learning was faster than expected for infants displaying both short looks and neutral…
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Attention, Infant Behavior, Infants