Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 9 |
Experiments | 9 |
Learning Strategies | 9 |
Models | 3 |
Problem Solving | 3 |
Classification | 2 |
Computation | 2 |
Difficulty Level | 2 |
Foreign Countries | 2 |
Hypothesis Testing | 2 |
Instructional Design | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 2 |
ALT-J: Research in Learning… | 1 |
Cognition and Instruction | 1 |
Cognitive Psychology | 1 |
Cognitive Science | 1 |
Educational Studies in… | 1 |
Journal of Experimental Child… | 1 |
Journal of Research on… | 1 |
Author
Bell, C. V. | 1 |
Chemla, Emmanuel | 1 |
Dautriche, Isabelle | 1 |
Lewandowsky, Stephan | 1 |
Lovett, Marsha C. | 1 |
Manches, Andrew | 1 |
Moller, Jens | 1 |
Muller-Kalthoff, Thiemo | 1 |
Nosofsky, Robert M. | 1 |
O'Malley, Claire | 1 |
Pape, S. J. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 4 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Grade 6 | 1 |
Grade 7 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Germany | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 1 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 1 |
Manches, Andrew; O'Malley, Claire – Cognition and Instruction, 2016
This article focuses on how the representational properties of manipulatives affect the strategies children employ in problem solving. Two studies examined the effect of physical materials on 4-7-year-old children's problem solving strategies in a numerical (i.e., additive composition) task. The first study showed how children not only identified…
Descriptors: Manipulative Materials, Object Manipulation, Young Children, Problem Solving
Dautriche, Isabelle; Chemla, Emmanuel – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Upon hearing a novel word, language learners must identify its correct meaning from a diverse set of situationally relevant options. Such referential ambiguity could be reduced through "repetitive" exposure to the novel word across diverging learning situations, a learning mechanism referred to as "cross-situational learning."…
Descriptors: Vocabulary Development, Language Acquisition, Ambiguity (Context), Ambiguity (Semantics)
Stanton, Roger D.; Nosofsky, Robert M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
Researchers have proposed that an explicit reasoning system is responsible for learning rule-based category structures and that a separate implicit, procedural-learning system is responsible for learning information-integration category structures. As evidence for this multiple-system hypothesis, researchers report a dissociation based on…
Descriptors: Classification, Psychological Studies, Learning Strategies, Cognitive Processes
Sewell, David K.; Lewandowsky, Stephan – Cognitive Psychology, 2011
Knowledge restructuring refers to changes in the strategy with which people solve a given problem. Two types of knowledge restructuring are supported by existing category learning models. The first is a relearning process, which involves incremental updating of knowledge as learning progresses. The second is a recoordination process, which…
Descriptors: Classification, Psychology, Cognitive Processes, Models
Star, Jon R.; Rittle-Johnson, Bethany – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
Comparing and contrasting examples is a core cognitive process that supports learning in children and adults across a variety of topics. In this experimental study, we evaluated the benefits of supporting comparison in a classroom context for children learning about computational estimation. Fifth- and sixth-grade students (N = 157) learned about…
Descriptors: Mathematics Education, Problem Solving, Teaching Methods, Cognitive Processes
Lovett, Marsha C. – Cognitive Science, 2005
Most accounts of the Stroop effect (Stroop, 1935) emphasize its negative aspect, namely, that in particular situations, processing of an irrelevant stimulus dimension interferes with participants' performance of the instructed task. In contrast, this paper emphasizes the fact that, even with that interference, participants actually can (and…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Models, Cognitive Processes, Prediction
Schar, Sissel Guttormsen; Zimmermann, Philippe G. – Journal of Research on Technology in Education, 2007
This paper covers an experiment designed to investigate the relationship between the didactical setting and learning effects with animations. We investigated whether the cognitive load imposed by animations could be reduced when the students could control the flow of the animation. We did not find an effect due to the fact that the students did…
Descriptors: Instructional Effectiveness, Knowledge Representation, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level
Muller-Kalthoff, Thiemo; Moller, Jens – ALT-J: Research in Learning Technology, 2006
One of the key reasons that multimedia, and particularly hypertext systems, are gaining in importance is that they inspire hopes of optimizing learners' processes of knowledge construction. The present study is concerned with the respective influence of individual learner variables (i.e. particularly domain-specific prior knowledge) on the use of…
Descriptors: Instructional Design, Prior Learning, Hypermedia, College Students
Pape, S. J.; Bell, C. V.; Yetkin, IE. – Educational Studies in Mathematics, 2003
Mathematics educators have found sociocultural models of teaching and learning to be powerful in their ability to describe and support the pursuit of instruction based on recent standards documents (e.g., National Council of Teachers of Mathematics [NCTM], 1989, 2000). These models of instruction, however, have been criticized for their lack of…
Descriptors: Mathematics Teachers, Mathematics Instruction, Learning Theories, Learning Strategies