NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 11 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Coene, Martine; Schauwers, Karen; Gillis, Steven; Rooryck, Johan; Govaerts, Paul J. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2011
Recent neurobiological studies have advanced the hypothesis that language development is not continuously plastic but is governed by biological constraints that may be modified by experience within a particular time window. This hypothesis is tested based on spontaneous speech data from deaf cochlear-implanted (CI) children with access to…
Descriptors: Genetics, Speech, Form Classes (Languages), Sensory Experience
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ferguson, Heather J.; Scheepers, Christoph; Sanford, Anthony J. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2010
During language comprehension, information about the world is exchanged and processed. Two essential ingredients of everyday cognition that are employed during language comprehension are the ability to reason counterfactually, and the ability to understand and predict other peoples' behaviour by attributing independent mental states to them…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Linguistic Input, Eye Movements, Beliefs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Scott, Rose M.; Fisher, Cynthia – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2009
Two-year-olds assign appropriate interpretations to verbs presented in two English transitivity alternations, the causal and unspecified-object alternations (Naigles, 1996). Here we explored how they might do so. Causal and unspecified-object verbs are syntactically similar. They can be either transitive or intransitive, but differ in the semantic…
Descriptors: Sentences, Cues, Semantics, Verbs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Arnold, Jennifer E.; Lao, Shin-Yi C. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2008
Research has shown that the comprehension of definite referring expressions (e.g., "the triangle") tends to be faster for "given" (previously mentioned) referents, compared with new referents. This has been attributed to the presence of given information in the consciousness of discourse participants (e.g., Chafe, 1994) suggesting that given is…
Descriptors: Word Order, Eye Movements, Reading Comprehension, Achievement
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
van der Lely, Heather K. J.; Ullman, Michael T. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2001
Evaluates the input-processing, deficit/single system and the grammar-specific deficit/dual system models to account for past tense formation in impaired and normal language development. Investigated regular and irregular past tense formation of 60 real and novel regular and irregular verbs in grammatical specifically language impaired (G)-SLI…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Grammar, Language Impairments, Linguistic Input
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dipper, Lucy T.; Black, Maria; Bryan, Karen L. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2005
In this paper, we reconsider some of the processes that distinguish production and comprehension. In particular, we discuss the specific forms of thinking involved in each: "thinking for speaking" and "thinking for listening" (Black and Chiat, 2000; Slobin, 1996). We argue that thinking for speaking (or for any form of language output) crucially…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Linguistic Input, Interaction, Language Impairments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Behrens, Heike – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2006
This study provides an account of the distributional information and the production rates in a particularly rich corpus of German child and adult language. Three structural domains are analysed: the parts-of-speech distribution for a coded corpus of circa one million words as well as the internal constituency of 300,000 noun phrases and almost…
Descriptors: Verbs, Nouns, Language Acquisition, German
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Romani, Cristina – Language and Cognitive Processes, 1992
An aphasic patient is described as one whose poor repetition of sentences and of lists of words contrasts with his or her surprisingly good performance on immediate problem recognition tasks. This result is interpreted as suggesting a distinction between phonological input and output buffers. (41 references) (Author/LB)
Descriptors: Aphasia, Communication Disorders, Comparative Analysis, Foreign Countries
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Morgan, James L. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 1996
Research presented in this paper on the character of infant-directed speech and the nature of infant speech perception abilities from 6 to 12 months suggests that prosody contributes significantly to early analyses of child languages and assists infants in developing root processes of parsing. (104 references) (Author/CK)
Descriptors: Auditory Perception, Auditory Stimuli, Child Language, Context Effect
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gaser, Michael; Smith, Linda B. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 1998
Proposes an alternative account of the child's learning of nouns and adjectives that relies on properties of the semantic categories to be learned and of the word-learning task itself. In five experiments, a simple connectionist network was trained to label input objects in particular contexts; the network learned categories resembling nouns…
Descriptors: Adjectives, Child Language, Language Acquisition, Language Patterns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Redington, Martin; Chater, Nick – Language and Cognitive Processes, 1998
Proposes that an important role for connectionist research in language acquisition is analyzing what linguistic information is present in the child's input. Recent connectionist and statistical work analyzing the properties of real language corpora suggest a priori objections against the utility of distributional information are misguided. This…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Language Processing, Language Research, Learning Processes