NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Publication Date
In 20250
Since 20240
Since 2021 (last 5 years)0
Since 2016 (last 10 years)0
Since 2006 (last 20 years)1
Education Level
Audience
Researchers31
Practitioners9
Teachers5
Policymakers1
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 31 results Save | Export
Eaton, Sarah Elaine – Online Submission, 2010
Today's language classroom is vastly different from that of the mid- to late 20th century. The study is a meta-analysis of recent research which provided the means to identify current and emerging trends in the field. Informed by this research, some identified trends that are shaping the 21st century language classroom are outdated practices such…
Descriptors: Language Laboratories, Teacher Attitudes, English (Second Language), Second Language Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hayes, Donald S.; And Others – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1987
The Flavell (l981) model of cognitive monitoring and metamnemonic development was tested by four experiments conducted to determine whether preschool children (1) recognize that mood, fatigue, and fear are variables that influence learning; and (2) self-monitor their internal states and adjust their study behavior when they are sad or tired. (NH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Learning Strategies, Memorization, Metacognition
Carlin, Michael T.; Soraci, Sal A.; Dennis, Nancy A.; Chechile, Nicholas A.; Loiselle, Raquel C. – American Journal on Mental Retardation, 2001
This study with 16 adolescents with mental retardation compared free-recall rates under two encoding conditions: (1) fade-in, initially presenting pictures out of focus then slowly fading them into focus; and (2) fade-out, slowly blurring originally clear pictures. Results indicated that free-recall rates were greater for the fade-in items for…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Cognitive Processes, Memorization, Memory
Warner, Michael M.; And Others – Learning Disabilities Research, 1989
The accuracy of recall and use of appropriate rehearsal strategies was compared for learning-disabled and low-achieving adolescents. There was little difference in performance between the two groups; both groups tended to employ appropriate executive strategies. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Learning Disabilities, Learning Strategies, Low Achievement, Memorization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Angelo, Jennifer Burgess – International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 1990
Two rule-based (truncation and contraction) abbreviation expansion encoding methods and 1 non-rule-based (random) method were evaluated for learning curve and retrieval speed with 66 adults. The truncation method resulted in faster retrieval than the other two methods. Results have implications for training users of augmentative communication…
Descriptors: Abbreviations, Coding, Communication Disorders, Learning Processes
PDF pending restoration PDF pending restoration
Morton, Kelly R.; Hall, Donald M. – 1983
The type of cognitive processing most beneficial to memory may depend on the structure of the material and the type of retrieval test required. Relational encoding organizes words by their relationships, while item-specific encoding processes words on an individual basis. To obtain information on these cognitive processes as they relate to the use…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, College Students, Higher Education, Memorization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hulme, Charles; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1986
Three experiments demonstrate that children four to ten years old, when presented with a series recall task with pictures of common objects having short or long names, showed consistently better recall of pictures with short names. (HOD)
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Development, Inner Speech (Subvocal), Memorization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lee, Carolyn P.; Obrzut, John E. – Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1994
This study investigated taxonomic clustering and use of frequency associations as features in the semantic memory of children (n=30 in grades two and six) with learning disabilities (LD). Results suggested that, when individual child-generated word lists (i.e., meaningful) are used, children with LD may not be impaired in their ability to utilize…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Processes, Elementary Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Wood, Eileen; Hewitt, Kathryn L. – Exceptionality: A Research Journal, 1993
Comparison of three learning strategies (elaborative interrogation, spontaneous strategy, or repetition control) with 53 high achievers in grades 5 and 6 found that both elaborative interrogation and spontaneous strategy conditions were equally effective and both were superior to the repetition condition. (DB)
Descriptors: Academically Gifted, High Achievement, Instructional Effectiveness, Intermediate Grades
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Melot, Anne-Marie; Corroyer, Denis – European Journal of Psychology of Education, 1992
Presents study results examining conditions in which children can apply a previously learned memorization strategy to new but analogous tasks. Concludes that strategy mastery during training is not sufficient. Reports that only subjects who use information about relations between procedures and results to construct generalizable knowledge are able…
Descriptors: Behavioral Science Research, Classification, Cognitive Style, Elementary Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Ackerman, Brian P. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1986
Describes four experiments to show that the effects of item-specific and relational encoding emphasis on recall vary with the retrieval context for both young children and adults. (HOD)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, College Students, Context Clues, Elementary School Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fulk, Barbara J. Mushinski; And Others – Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 1992
This study with 56 learning-disabled adolescents found that intensive generalization training specific to the development of complex mnemonic strategies was demonstrably more effective in recall at 1-day and 2-week intervals than a rehearsal condition. No added advantage was gained by adding attribution training to the mnemonic generalization…
Descriptors: Generalization, Instructional Effectiveness, Learning Disabilities, Learning Strategies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Nesbit, John C.; Hunka, Steve – Instructional Science, 1987
Proposes a criterion for selecting a sequence of objectives from a learning hierarchy and describes a sequence generating algorithm which is designed to generate a minimal memory load sequence from a learning tree. Its effectiveness in generating low memory load sequences from hierarchies that are not trees is also discussed. (RP)
Descriptors: Algorithms, Computer Assisted Instruction, Educational Objectives, Instructional Design
Cornbleth, Catherine – 1986
Myth is integral to modern as well as ancient societies in its service to multiple interrelated social functions. Among these are to explain phenomena and direct action, to justify particular interests or practices, to dramatize ideals, and to provide cultural cohesion. Three prevailing myths that are especially problematic are the myths of…
Descriptors: Christianity, Cognitive Processes, Curriculum Design, Developmental Stages
Lohman, David F. – 1984
Although the distinction between verbal ability and spatial ability is fundamental in all models of human abilities, differences in the relative strengths of verbal and spatial abilities have failed to show consistent interactions with instructional treatments. This study investigated the hypothesis that spatial tests measure different abilities…
Descriptors: Aptitude Treatment Interaction, Confidence Testing, Higher Education, Memorization
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3