NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 6 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dube, Chad; Starns, Jeffrey J.; Rotello, Caren M.; Ratcliff, Roger – Journal of Memory and Language, 2012
A classic question in the recognition memory literature is whether retrieval is best described as a continuous-evidence process consistent with signal detection theory (SDT), or a threshold process consistent with many multinomial processing tree (MPT) models. Because receiver operating characteristics (ROCs) based on confidence ratings are…
Descriptors: Recognition (Psychology), Reaction Time, Perception, Bias
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McKoon, Gail; Ratcliff, Roger – Journal of Memory and Language, 2013
In the domain of discourse processing, it has been claimed that older adults (60-0-year-olds) are less likely to encode and remember some kinds of information from texts than young adults. The experiment described here shows that they do make a particular kind of inference to the same extent that college-age adults do. The inferences examined were…
Descriptors: Accuracy, Theory Practice Relationship, Young Adults, Inferences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Staub, Adrian – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Speakers frequently make subject-verb number agreement errors in the presence of a local noun with a different number from the head of the subject phrase. A series of four experiments used a two-choice response time (RT) paradigm to investigate how the latency of correct agreement decisions is modulated by the presence of a number attractor, and…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Nouns, Syntax, Error Patterns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Nozari, Nazbanou; Dell, Gary S. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
The lexical bias effect (the tendency for phonological speech errors to create words more often than nonwords) has been debated for over 30 years. One account attributes the effect to a lexical editor, a strategic component of the production system that examines each planned phonological string, and suppresses it if it is a nonword. The…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Oral Language, Editing, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bloem, Ineke; van den Boogaard, Sylvia; Heij, Wido La – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
Bloem and La Heij (2003) reported that in a word-translation task context words induce semantic interference whereas context pictures induce semantic facilitation. This finding was accounted for by a model of lexical access in which: (a) semantic facilitation is localized at the conceptual level, (b) semantic interference is localized at the…
Descriptors: Semantics, Interference (Language), Models, Phonology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Perea, Manuel; Lupker, Stephen J. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2004
Nonwords created by transposing two "adjacent" letters (i.e., transposed-letter (TL) nonwords like "jugde") are very effective at activating the lexical representation of their base words. This fact poses problems for most computational models of word recognition (e.g., the interactive-activation model and its extensions), which assume that exact…
Descriptors: Alphabets, Word Recognition, Models, Lexicology