NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 11 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McMurray, Bob; Horst, Jessica S.; Samuelson, Larissa K. – Psychological Review, 2012
Classic approaches to word learning emphasize referential ambiguity: In naming situations, a novel word could refer to many possible objects, properties, actions, and so forth. To solve this, researchers have posited constraints, and inference strategies, but assume that determining the referent of a novel word is isomorphic to learning. We…
Descriptors: Word Recognition, Vocabulary Development, Language Acquisition, Interaction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yu, Chen; Smith, Linda B. – Psychological Review, 2012
Both adults and young children possess powerful statistical computation capabilities--they can infer the referent of a word from highly ambiguous contexts involving many words and many referents by aggregating cross-situational statistical information across contexts. This ability has been explained by models of hypothesis testing and by models of…
Descriptors: Testing, Associative Learning, Hypothesis Testing, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Eidels, Ami; Townsend, James T.; Algom, Daniel – Cognition, 2010
A huge set of focused attention experiments show that when presented with color words printed in color, observers report the ink color faster if the carrier word is the name of the color rather than the name of an alternative color, the Stroop effect. There is also a large number (although not so numerous as the Stroop task) of so-called…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Processes, Color, Associative Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Regier, Terry – Cognitive Science, 2005
Children improve at word learning during the 2nd year of life--sometimes dramatically. This fact has suggested a change in mechanism, from associative learning to a more referential form of learning. This article presents an associative exemplar-based model that accounts for the improvement without a change in mechanism. It provides a unified…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Models, Semantics, Phonology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Healy, Michael R.; Light, Leah L.; Chung, Christie – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2005
In 3 experiments, young and older adults studied lists of unrelated word pairs and were given confidence-rated item and associative recognition tests. Several different models of recognition were fit to the confidence-rating data using techniques described by S. Macho (2002, 2004). Concordant with previous findings, item recognition data were best…
Descriptors: Models, Young Adults, Older Adults, Experiments
Snodgrass, Joan Gay – 1979
A model was developed to account for similarities and differences between picture and word processing in a variety of semantic and episodic memory tasks. The model contains three levels of processing: low-level processing of the physical characteristics of externally presented pictures and words; an intermediate level where the low-level processor…
Descriptors: Association (Psychology), Associative Learning, Cognitive Processes, Memory
Kleiman, Glenn M. – 1979
Two experiments explored whether the facilitatory effect of context on lexical decisions is limited to words subjects generated when given the context as a prompt in a production task, or if the effect is wider in scope. The first experiment provided evidence of a wide scope of facilitation from single word contexts. In the second experiment, the…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Concept Formation, Context Clues, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Katz, Leonard; Baldasare, John – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1983
Phonological coding in printed-word recognition in English was studied by examining the use made of syllable information by skilled and less skilled readers in the second grade and by adults. The results are discussed in terms of an interactive model of reading. (Author/PN)
Descriptors: Adults, Association (Psychology), Associative Learning, Comparative Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Mason, Mildred – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1982
Three experiments report additional evidence that it is a mistake to account for all interletter effects solely in terms of sensory variables. These experiments attest to the importance of structural variables such as retina location, array size, and ordinal position. (Author/PN)
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Cognitive Processes, Eye Fixations, Higher Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Eisenberg, Peter; Becker, Curtis A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1982
Individual differences in context effects both in a word-level task and in a sentence-level task were found to be related to individual differences in reading continuous text. These results are presented within the framework of a verification model, and the implications for two-process theory are discussed. (Author/PN)
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Attention, Context Clues, Interference (Language)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
de Groot, Annette M. B.; Comijs, Hannie – Language Learning, 1995
Explores the "translation-recognition" task, using two experiments, one for adult Dutch learners of English and the second for people from the same population. Results suggest that translation recognition and translation production respond to the same manipulations, except when cognates and noncognates are focused on separately. (23…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Bilingualism, College Students, Comparative Analysis