Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1 |
Descriptor
Motion | 6 |
Infants | 5 |
Age Differences | 4 |
Visual Perception | 4 |
Visual Stimuli | 4 |
Perceptual Development | 3 |
Comparative Analysis | 2 |
Infant Behavior | 2 |
Attention | 1 |
Biomechanics | 1 |
Cognitive Development | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Bertenthal, Bennett I. | 6 |
Booth, Amy E. | 1 |
Bradbury, Anne | 1 |
Brand, Rebecca | 1 |
Chung, He Len | 1 |
Eizenman, Dara R. | 1 |
Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick | 1 |
Hennon, Elizabeth | 1 |
Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy | 1 |
Liu, Jing | 1 |
Longo, Matthew R. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Rohlfing, Katharina J.; Longo, Matthew R.; Bertenthal, Bennett I. – Developmental Science, 2012
Pointing, like eye gaze, is a deictic gesture that can be used to orient the attention of another person towards an object or an event. Previous research suggests that infants first begin to follow a pointing gesture between 10 and 13 months of age. We investigated whether sensitivity to pointing could be seen at younger ages employing a technique…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Attention, Infants, Motion

Booth, Amy E.; Pinto, Jeannine; Bertenthal, Bennett I. – Developmental Psychology, 2002
Two experiments tested infants' sensitivity to properties of point-light displays of a walker and a runner that were equivalent regarding the phasing of limb movements. Found that 3-, but not 5-month-olds, discriminated these displays. When the symmetrical phase-patterning of the runner display was perturbed by advancing two of its limbs by 25…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Infants, Motion, Perceptual Development

Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; Chung, He Len; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Liu, Jing; Bertenthal, Bennett I.; Brand, Rebecca; Maguire, Mandy J.; Hennon, Elizabeth – Developmental Psychology, 2002
Used point-light displays (lights corresponding to the joints of the human body) to examine 3-year-olds' understanding of verbs. Found that children could extend familiar motion verbs (walking, dancing) to videotaped point-light actions shown in an intermodal preferential looking paradigm. Children watched the action matching the requested verb…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Comparative Analysis, Generalization, Motion

Bertenthal, Bennett I.; Bradbury, Anne – Developmental Psychology, 1992
Assessed 13- and 20-week-olds infants' discrimination between shearing stimuli, in which columns of dots move vertically on a screen at different velocities, and foil stimuli, in which all dots move at the same velocity. Results revealed the threshold levels of dot velocity in shearing stimuli at which discrimination occurred. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Infants, Motion, Perceptual Development

Bertenthal, Bennett I.; And Others – Child Development, 1985
Examines, in three experiments, infant sensitivity at 20, 30, and 36 weeks of age to 3-dimensional structure of a human form specified through biomechanical motions. Findings are interpreted as suggesting that infants, by 36 weeks of age, are extracting fundamental properties necessary for interpreting a point-light display as a person. (Author/BE)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Biomechanics, Cognitive Processes, Dimensional Preference

Eizenman, Dara R.; Bertenthal, Bennett I. – Developmental Psychology, 1998
Three experiments examined 4- and 6-month-olds' sensitivity to the unity of a partly occluded moving rod undergoing translation, rotation, or oscillation. Findings suggested that all types of common motion were not equivalent for specifying infants' perceptions of occluded objects. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Comparative Analysis, Infant Behavior, Infants