NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 10 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ding, Meixia; Li, Xiaobao; Capraro, Mary M. – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2013
This study examines preservice elementary teachers' (PTs) knowledge for teaching the associative property (AP) of multiplication. Results reveal that PTs hold a common misconception between the AP and commutative property (CP). Most PTs in our sample were unable to use concrete contexts (e.g., pictorial representations and word problems) to…
Descriptors: Preservice Teachers, Elementary School Teachers, Multiplication, Elementary School Mathematics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Speiser, Bob; Walter, Chuck – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2011
This paper explores how models can support productive thinking. For us a model is a "thing", a tool to help make sense of something. We restrict attention to specific models for whole-number multiplication, hence the wording of the title. They support evolving thinking in large measure through the ways their users redesign them. They assume new…
Descriptors: Models, Productive Thinking, Concept Formation, Fundamental Concepts
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Muzheve, Michael T.; Capraro, Robert M. – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2012
Using qualitative data collection and analyses techniques, we examined mathematical representations used by sixteen (N=16) teachers while teaching the concepts of converting among fractions, decimals, and percents. We also studied representational choices by their students (N=581). In addition to using geometric figures and manipulatives, teachers…
Descriptors: Geometric Concepts, Mathematics, Misconceptions, Natural Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Caddle, Mary C.; Brizuela, Barbara M. – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2011
This paper looks at 21 fifth grade students as they discuss a linear graph in the Cartesian plane. The problem presented to students depicted a graph showing distance as a function of elapsed time for a person walking at a constant rate of 5 miles/h. The question asked students to consider how many more hours, after having already walked 4 h,…
Descriptors: Grade 5, Mathematics Instruction, Elementary School Students, Mathematical Logic
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Speiser, Robert; Schneps, Matthew H.; Heffner-Wong, Amanda; Miller, Jaimie L.; Sonnert, Gerhard – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2012
In school, at least in the US, we were taught to multiply by hand according to a standard algorithm. Most people find that algorithm difficult to use, and many children fail to learn it. We propose a new way to make sense of this difficulty: to treat explicit computation as perceptually supported physical and mental action. Based on recent work in…
Descriptors: Evidence, Mathematics, Urban Schools, Short Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hackenberg, Amy J.; Tillema, Erik S. – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2009
This article reports on the activity of two pairs of sixth grade students who participated in an 8-month teaching experiment that investigated the students' construction of fraction composition schemes. A fraction composition scheme consists of the operations and concepts used to determine, for example, the size of 1/3 of 1/5 of a whole in…
Descriptors: Numbers, Concept Formation, Grade 6, Mathematics Instruction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ramful, Ajay; Olive, John – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2008
In line with current efforts to understand the piece-by-piece structure and articulation of children's mathematical concepts, this case study compares the reversibility schemes of two eighth-grade students. The aim of the study was to identify the mechanism through which students reverse their thought processes in a multiplicative situation. Data…
Descriptors: Mathematical Concepts, Cognitive Processes, Multiplication, Case Studies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Empson, Susan B.; Turner, Erin – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2006
Although children partition by repeatedly halving easily and spontaneously as early as the age of 4, multiplicative thinking is difficult and develops over a long period in school. Given the apparently multiplicative character of repeated halving and doubling, it is natural to ask what role they might play in the development of multiplicative…
Descriptors: Mathematics Instruction, Mathematical Logic, Thinking Skills, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lampert, Magdalene – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 1986
How multiplication is usually taught in school and how it could be taught are discussed. Development of understanding is illustrated through children's words and work. (MNS)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Concept Formation, Elementary Education, Elementary School Mathematics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Schliemann, Analucia Dias; And Others – Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 1993
A 14-year-old Brazilian boy in the fourth grade was given 170 problems orally, each asking for the sum of 2 2-digit numbers. Responses indicated he had invented his own algorithm for addition, similar to the school one, based largely on his experiences with money. (Contains 13 references.) (JAF)
Descriptors: Addition, Algorithms, Case Studies, Computation