Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Navigation | 9 |
Short Term Memory | 9 |
Spatial Ability | 8 |
Cognitive Processes | 7 |
Hypothesis Testing | 3 |
Task Analysis | 3 |
Accuracy | 2 |
Adults | 2 |
Animals | 2 |
Children | 2 |
Cognitive Mapping | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Learning & Memory | 2 |
Applied Cognitive Psychology | 1 |
Cognitive Science | 1 |
Developmental Science | 1 |
Journal of Autism and… | 1 |
Journal of Experimental Child… | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Journal of Problem Solving | 1 |
Author
Altgassen, Mareike | 1 |
Ascoli, Giorgio A. | 1 |
Barr, Peter | 1 |
Baumann, Cédric | 1 |
Belmonti, Vittorio | 1 |
Berthoz, Alain | 1 |
Blades, Mark | 1 |
Bowler, Dermot M. | 1 |
Chu, Yun | 1 |
Cioni, Giovanni | 1 |
Clerc-Urmes, Isabelle | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 9 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 3 |
Postsecondary Education | 3 |
Audience
Location
Japan (Tokyo) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Autism Diagnostic Observation… | 1 |
Wechsler Adult Intelligence… | 1 |
Wide Range Achievement Test | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Jordan, Jake T.; Tong, Yi; Pytte, Carolyn L. – Learning & Memory, 2022
Plasticity is a neural phenomenon in which experience induces long-lasting changes to neuronal circuits and is at the center of most neurobiological theories of learning and memory. However, too much plasticity is maladaptive and must be balanced with substrate stability. Area CA3 of the hippocampus provides such a balance via hemispheric…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Memory, Learning Processes
Jacob, Christel; Rainville, Constant; Trognon, Alain; Fescharek, Reinhard; Baumann, Cédric; Clerc-Urmes, Isabelle; Rivasseau Jonveaux, Thérèse – Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2020
The cognitive processes involved in route retracing are not well known. This study aims to highlight them in an elderly population in which contradictory results have been obtained, certain studies showing specific difficulties for route retracing, others not. Thirty-nine elderly subjects performed a route-learning task (forward-backward) in a…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Cognitive Processes, Navigation, Older Adults
Ring, Melanie; Gaigg, Sebastian B.; Altgassen, Mareike; Barr, Peter; Bowler, Dermot M. – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2018
Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) present difficulties in forming relations among items and context. This capacity for relational binding is also involved in spatial navigation and research on this topic in ASD is scarce and inconclusive. Using a computerised version of the Morris Water Maze task, ASD participants showed particular…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Short Term Memory, Adults, Autism
Gardner, Robert S.; Uttaro, Michael R.; Fleming, Samantha E.; Suarez, Daniel F.; Ascoli, Giorgio A.; Dumas, Theodore C. – Learning & Memory, 2013
Learning by repetition engages distinct cognitive strategies whose contributions are adjusted with experience. Early in learning, performance relies upon flexible, attentive strategies. With extended practice, inflexible, automatic strategies emerge. This transition is thought fundamental to habit formation and applies to human and animal…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Learning Strategies, Spatial Ability, Navigation
Belmonti, Vittorio; Cioni, Giovanni; Berthoz, Alain – Developmental Science, 2015
Navigational and reaching spaces are known to involve different cognitive strategies and brain networks, whose development in humans is still debated. In fact, high-level spatial processing, including allocentric location encoding, is already available to very young children, but navigational strategies are not mature until late childhood. The…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Navigation, Spatial Ability, Hypothesis Testing
Weisberg, Steven M.; Newcombe, Nora S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Research on the existence of cognitive maps and on the cognitive processes that support effective navigation has often focused on functioning across individuals. However, there are pronounced individual differences in navigation proficiency, which need to be explained and which can illuminate our understanding of cognitive maps and effective…
Descriptors: Cognitive Mapping, Cognitive Processes, Short Term Memory, Individual Differences
Wen, Wen; Ishikawa, Toru; Sato, Takao – Cognitive Science, 2013
This study examined how different components of working memory are involved in the acquisition of egocentric and allocentric survey knowledge by people with a good and poor sense of direction (SOD). We employed a dual-task method and asked participants to learn routes from videos with verbal, visual, and spatial interference tasks and without any…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Cognitive Processes, Short Term Memory, Spatial Ability
Purser, Harry R. M.; Farran, Emily K.; Courbois, Yannick; Lemahieu, Axelle; Mellier, Daniel; Sockeel, Pascal; Blades, Mark – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The aim of this study was to investigate route-learning ability in 67 children aged 5 to 11 years and to relate route-learning performance to the components of Baddeley's model of working memory. Children carried out tasks that included measures of verbal and visuospatial short-term memory and executive control and also measures of verbal and…
Descriptors: Virtual Classrooms, Short Term Memory, Long Term Memory, Children
Chu, Yun; Li, Zheng; Su, Yong; Pizlo, Zygmunt – Journal of Problem Solving, 2010
Isomorphs of a puzzle called m+m resulted in faster solution times and an easily reproduced solution path in a labeled version of the problem compared to a more difficult binary version. We conjecture that performance is related to a type of heuristic called direction that not only constrains search space in the labeled version, but also…
Descriptors: Heuristics, Problem Solving, Puzzles, Navigation