Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Neonates | 9 |
Visual Stimuli | 6 |
Visual Perception | 5 |
Child Development | 3 |
Habituation | 3 |
Cognitive Processes | 2 |
Experiments | 2 |
Infant Behavior | 2 |
Motion | 2 |
Stimuli | 2 |
Visual Discrimination | 2 |
More ▼ |
Author
Valenza, Eloisa | 9 |
Gava, Lucia | 4 |
Bulf, Hermann | 3 |
Leo, Irene | 3 |
Turati, Chiara | 3 |
Simion, Francesca | 2 |
Cassia, Viola Macchi | 1 |
Ghirardi, Valentina | 1 |
Johnson, Scott P. | 1 |
Zulian, Luisa | 1 |
de Schonen, Scania | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Turati, Chiara; Gava, Lucia; Valenza, Eloisa; Ghirardi, Valentina – Cognitive Development, 2013
This study investigated processing of number and extent in newborns. Using visual preference, we showed that newborns discriminated between small sets of dot collections relying solely on implicit numerical information when non-numerical continuous variables were strictly controlled (Experiment 1), and solely on continuous information when…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Attention, Neonates, Numbers
Bulf, Hermann; Johnson, Scott P.; Valenza, Eloisa – Cognition, 2011
Statistical learning--implicit learning of statistical regularities within sensory input--is a way of acquiring structure within continuous sensory environments. Statistics computation, initially shown to be involved in word segmentation, has been demonstrated to be a general mechanism that operates across domains, across time and space, and…
Descriptors: Neonates, Statistics, Sensory Experience, Visual Perception
Valenza, Eloisa; Bulf, Hermann – Developmental Science, 2011
The present study aimed to investigate whether perceptual completion is available at birth, in the absence of any visual experience. An extremely underspecified kinetic visual display composed of four spatially separated fragments arranged to give rise to an illusory rectangle that occluded a vertical rod (illusory condition) or rotated so as not…
Descriptors: Evidence, Stimuli, Mechanics (Physics), Neonates
Gava, Lucia; Valenza, Eloisa; Turati, Chiara – Child Development, 2009
Five experiments examined 79 newborns' ability to discriminate and categorize a spatial relation, defined by the left-right spatial position of a blinking object-target with respect to a vertical landmark-bar. Three-day-old infants discriminated the up versus low position of an object located on the same side of the landmark-bar (Experiment 1) and…
Descriptors: Neonates, Spatial Ability, Visual Stimuli, Visual Discrimination
Cassia, Viola Macchi; Valenza, Eloisa; Simion, Francesca; Leo, Irene – Child Development, 2008
Past research has shown that top-heaviness is a perceptual property that plays a crucial role in triggering newborns' preference toward faces. The present study examined the contribution of a second configural property, "congruency," to newborns' face preference. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that when embedded in nonfacelike stimuli,…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Neonates, Infant Behavior, Visual Stimuli
Gava, Lucia; Valenza, Eloisa; Turati, Chiara; de Schonen, Scania – Developmental Science, 2008
Many studies have shown that newborns prefer (e.g. Goren, Sarty & Wu, 1975 ; Valenza, Simion, Macchi Cassia & Umilta, 1996) and recognize (e.g. Bushnell, Say & Mullin, 1989; Pascalis & de Schonen, 1994) faces. However, it is not known whether, at birth, faces are still preferred and recognized when some of their parts are not visible because…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Neonates, Recognition (Psychology), Child Development
Valenza, Eloisa; Bulf, Hermann – Developmental Science, 2007
Previous research, in which static figures were used, showed that the ability to perceive illusory contours emerges around 7 months of age. However, recently, evidence has suggested that 2-3-month-old infants are able to perceive illusory contours when motion information is available (Johnson & Mason, 2002; Otsuka & Yamaguchi, 2003). The present…
Descriptors: Neonates, Motion, Kinetics, Kinesthetic Perception
Valenza, Eloisa; Leo, Irene; Gava, Lucia; Simion, Francesca – Child Development, 2006
Despite decades of studies of human infants, a still open question concerns the role of visual experience in the development of the ability to perceive complete shapes over partial occlusion. Previous studies show that newborns fail to manifest this ability, either because they lack the visual experience required for perceptual completion or…
Descriptors: Neonates, Visual Perception, Motion
Valenza, Eloisa; Zulian, Luisa; Leo, Irene – Infancy, 2005
This study explored whether the reported inability of newborns to perceive object unity could result from the limited abilities of newborns to recognize the correspondence between 2 stimuli that were identical except for the presence or absence of an occluder. Five experiments were carried out using a visual habituation technique. The results of…
Descriptors: Neonates, Visual Perception, Visual Stimuli, Experiments