Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 53 |
Descriptor
Neurological Organization | 53 |
Brain | 26 |
Cognitive Processes | 22 |
Brain Hemisphere Functions | 13 |
Memory | 13 |
Language Processing | 10 |
Semantics | 10 |
Diagnostic Tests | 9 |
Prediction | 7 |
Comparative Analysis | 6 |
Role | 6 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Cognitive… | 53 |
Author
Cabeza, Roberto | 2 |
Cohen, Michael X. | 2 |
Gliga, Teodora | 2 |
Kherif, Ferath | 2 |
Neville, Helen J. | 2 |
Pakulak, Eric | 2 |
Rodriguez-Fornells, Antoni | 2 |
Shohamy, Daphna | 2 |
Akeson, Per | 1 |
Alexander, Michael P. | 1 |
Ali, Nilufa | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 53 |
Reports - Research | 45 |
Reports - Evaluative | 7 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Haegens, Saskia; Luther, Lisa; Jensen, Ole – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2012
Effective processing of sensory input in daily life requires attentional selection and amplification of relevant input and, just as importantly, attenuation of irrelevant information. It has been proposed that top-down modulation of oscillatory alpha band activity (8-14 Hz) serves to allocate resources to various regions, depending on task…
Descriptors: Attention, Tactual Perception, Cues, Testing
Laird, Angela R.; Fox, P. Mickle; Eickhoff, Simon B.; Turner, Jessica A.; Ray, Kimberly L.; McKay, D. Reese; Glahn, David C.; Beckmann, Christian F.; Smith, Stephen M.; Fox, Peter T. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
An increasingly large number of neuroimaging studies have investigated functionally connected networks during rest, providing insight into human brain architecture. Assessment of the functional qualities of resting state networks has been limited by the task-independent state, which results in an inability to relate these networks to specific…
Descriptors: Brain, Neurological Organization, Cognitive Processes, Behavior
Chandrasekaran, Bharath; Chan, Alice H. D.; Wong, Patrick C. M. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Human speech is composed of two types of information, related to content (lexical information, i.e., "what" is being said [e.g., words]) and to the speaker (indexical information, i.e., "who" is talking [e.g., voices]). The extent to which lexical versus indexical information is represented separately or integrally in the brain is unresolved. In…
Descriptors: Speech, Cognitive Processes, Brain, Neurological Organization
Zacks, Jeffrey M.; Kurby, Christopher A.; Eisenberg, Michelle L.; Haroutunian, Nayiri – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Predicting the near future is important for survival and plays a central role in theories of perception, language processing, and learning. Prediction failures may be particularly important for initiating the updating of perceptual and memory systems and, thus, for the subjective experience of events. Here, we asked observers to make predictions…
Descriptors: Prediction, Perception, Memory, Neurological Organization
Burke, Melanie Rose; Barnes, Graham R. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
A previous study has shown that actively pursuing a moving target provides a predictive motor advantage when compared with passive observation of the moving target while keeping the eyes still [Burke, M. R., & Barnes, G. R. Anticipatory eye movements evoked after active following versus passive observation of a predictable motion stimulus. "Brain…
Descriptors: Observation, Eye Movements, Brain, Inhibition
Peramunage, Dasun; Blumstein, Sheila E.; Myers, Emily B.; Goldrick, Matthew; Baese-Berk, Melissa – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
The current study examined the neural systems underlying lexically conditioned phonetic variation in spoken word production. Participants were asked to read aloud singly presented words, which either had a voiced minimal pair (MP) neighbor (e.g., cape) or lacked a minimal pair (NMP) neighbor (e.g., cake). The voiced neighbor never appeared in the…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Speech, Phonology, Phonetics
Vetter, Petra; Butterworth, Brian; Bahrami, Bahador – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Several recent behavioral studies have shown that the enumeration of a small number of items (a process termed "subitizing") depends on the availability of attentional resources and is not a preattentive process as previously thought. Here we studied the neural correlates of visual enumeration under different attentional loads in a dual-task…
Descriptors: Attention, Numbers, Neurological Organization, Short Term Memory
van de Vijver, Irene; Ridderinkhof, K. Richard; Cohen, Michael X. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Frontal oscillatory dynamics in the theta (4-8 Hz) and beta (20-30 Hz) frequency bands have been implicated in cognitive control processes. Here we investigated the changes in coordinated activity within and between frontal brain areas during feedback-based response learning. In a time estimation task, participants learned to press a button after…
Descriptors: Brain, Neurological Organization, Cognitive Processes, Feedback (Response)
Hayes, Scott M.; Buchler, Norbou; Stokes, Jared; Kragel, James; Cabeza, Roberto – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Although the medial-temporal lobes (MTL), PFC, and parietal cortex are considered primary nodes in the episodic memory network, there is much debate regarding the contributions of MTL, PFC, and parietal subregions to recollection versus familiarity (dual-process theory) and the feasibility of accounts on the basis of a single memory strength…
Descriptors: Recognition (Psychology), Memory, Theories, Neurological Organization
Cowan, Nelson; Li, Dawei; Moffitt, Amanda; Becker, Theresa M.; Martin, Elizabeth A.; Saults, J. Scott; Christ, Shawn E. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Over 350 years ago, Descartes proposed that the neural basis of consciousness must be a brain region in which sensory inputs are combined. Using fMRI, we identified at least one such area for working memory, the limited information held in mind, described by William James as the trailing edge of consciousness. Specifically, a region in the left…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Brain, Neurological Organization, Visual Stimuli
de Diego-Balaguer, Ruth; Fuentemilla, Lluis; Rodriguez-Fornells, Antoni – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Language acquisition is a complex process that requires the synergic involvement of different cognitive functions, which include extracting and storing the words of the language and their embedded rules for progressive acquisition of grammatical information. As has been shown in other fields that study learning processes, synchronization…
Descriptors: Brain, Speech, Language Acquisition, Artificial Languages
Anderson, John R.; Bothell, Daniel; Fincham, Jon M.; Anderson, Abraham R.; Poole, Ben; Qin, Yulin – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Part- and whole-task conditions were created by manipulating the presence of certain components of the Space Fortress video game. A cognitive model was created for two-part games that could be combined into a model that performed the whole game. The model generated predictions both for behavioral patterns and activation patterns in various brain…
Descriptors: Video Games, Brain, Neurological Organization, Models
Shaw, Daniel J.; Grosbras, Marie-Helene; Leonard, Gabriel; Pike, G. Bruce; Paus, Tomas – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2011
Successful interpersonal interactions rely on an ability to read the emotional states of others and to modulate one's own behavior in response. The actions of others serve as valuable social stimuli in this respect, offering the observer an insight into the actor's emotional state. Social cognition continues to mature throughout adolescence. Here…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Interpersonal Relationship, Social Cognition, Social Behavior
Shetreet, Einat; Friedmann, Naama; Hadar, Uri – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2010
Unaccusative verbs like "fall" are special in that their sole argument is syntactically generated at the object position of the verb rather than at the subject position. Unaccusative verbs are derived by a lexical operation that reduces the agent from transitive verbs. Their insertion into a sentence often involves a syntactic movement from the…
Descriptors: Brain, Verbs, Linguistics, Syntax
Ali, Nilufa; Green, David W.; Kherif, Ferath; Devlin, Joseph T.; Price, Cathy J. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2010
Suppressing irrelevant words is essential to successful speech production and is expected to involve general control mechanisms that reduce interference from task-unrelated processing. To investigate the neural mechanisms that suppress visual word interference, we used fMRI and a Stroop task, using a block design with an event-related analysis.…
Descriptors: Speech, Language Processing, Color, Brain