Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Comparative Analysis | 2 |
Foreign Countries | 2 |
Nonverbal Communication | 2 |
Sign Language | 2 |
Age Differences | 1 |
Case Studies | 1 |
Check Lists | 1 |
Child Language | 1 |
Children | 1 |
Cues | 1 |
Infants | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 2 |
Author
Burtenshaw, Tiarn | 1 |
Fay, Nicolas | 1 |
Fletcher, Ben C. | 1 |
Howlett, Neil | 1 |
Kirk, Elizabeth | 1 |
Lister, Casey J. | 1 |
Ohan, Jeneva L. | 1 |
Pine, Karen J. | 1 |
Walker, Bradley | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Australia | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Preschool Language Scale | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Lister, Casey J.; Burtenshaw, Tiarn; Walker, Bradley; Ohan, Jeneva L.; Fay, Nicolas – Child Development, 2021
Naturalistic studies show that children can create language-like communication systems in the absence of conventional language. However, experimental evidence is mixed. We address this discrepancy using an experimental paradigm that simulates naturalistic sign creation. Specifically, we tested if a sample of 6- to 12-year-old children (52 girls…
Descriptors: Case Studies, Sign Language, Nonverbal Communication, Comparative Analysis
Kirk, Elizabeth; Howlett, Neil; Pine, Karen J.; Fletcher, Ben C. – Child Development, 2013
Findings are presented from the first randomized control trial of the effects of encouraging symbolic gesture (or "baby sign") on infant language, following 40 infants from age 8 months to 20 months. Half of the mothers were trained to model a target set of gestures to their infants. Frequent measures were taken of infant language…
Descriptors: Infants, Sign Language, Language Acquisition, Child Language