Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 2 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 5 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 8 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 19 |
Descriptor
Cues | 21 |
Nonverbal Communication | 21 |
Eye Movements | 10 |
Child Development | 8 |
Infants | 7 |
Interpersonal Communication | 6 |
Toddlers | 6 |
Age Differences | 5 |
Cognitive Processes | 5 |
Human Body | 5 |
Task Analysis | 5 |
More ▼ |
Source
Developmental Science | 21 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 21 |
Reports - Research | 17 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Intermediate Grades | 1 |
Audience
Location
North America | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
The Slow Emergence of Gaze- and Point-Following: A Longitudinal Study of Infants from 4 to 12 Months
Yueyan Tang; Marybel Robledo Gonzalez; Gedeon O. Deák – Developmental Science, 2024
Acquisition of visual attention-following skills, notably gaze- and point-following, contributes to infants' ability to share attention with caregivers, which in turn contributes to social learning and communication. However, the development of gaze- and point-following in the first 18 months remains controversial, in part because of different…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Nonverbal Communication, Longitudinal Studies, Infants
Kliesch, Christian; Parise, Eugenio; Reid, Vincent; Hoehl, Stefanie – Developmental Science, 2022
Learning about actions requires children to identify the boundaries of an action and its units. Whereas some action units are easily identified, parents can support children's action learning by adjusting the presentation and using social signals. However, currently, little is understood regarding how children use these signals to learn actions.…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Imitation, Learning Processes, Interpersonal Communication
Antonya M. Gonzalez; Allison L. Skinner; Andrew Scott Baron – Developmental Science, 2025
Nonverbal behavior is a ubiquitous, everyday cue that is often used as a basis for social evaluation. Numerous studies indicate that children are sensitive to these signals and form evaluative judgments after viewing positive or negative nonverbal cues directed toward a target. Furthermore, they generalize these judgments to other members of a…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Grade 4, Nonverbal Communication, Childrens Attitudes
Cheung, Rachael W.; Hartley, Calum; Monaghan, Padraic – Developmental Science, 2021
Children learn words in environments where there is considerable variability, both in terms of the number of possible referents for novel words, and the availability of cues to support word-referent mappings. How caregivers adapt their gestural cues to referential uncertainty has not yet been explored. We tested a computational model of…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Vocabulary Development, Cues, Caregiver Role
Chen, Chi-hsin; Castellanos, Irina; Yu, Chen; Houston, Derek M. – Developmental Science, 2020
Coordinated attention between children and their parents plays an important role in their social, language, and cognitive development. The current study used head-mounted eye-trackers to investigate the effects of children's prelingual hearing loss on how they achieve coordinated attention with their hearing parents during free-flowing object…
Descriptors: Attention, Parent Child Relationship, Child Development, Eye Movements
Esteve-Gibert, Núria; Loevenbruck, Hélène; Dohen, Marion; D'Imperio, Mariapaola – Developmental Science, 2022
Previous evidence suggests that children's mastery of prosodic modulations to signal the informational status of discourse referents emerges quite late in development. In the present study, we investigate the children's use of head gestures as it compares to prosodic cues to signal a referent as being contrastive relative to a set of possible…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Nonverbal Communication, Intonation, Suprasegmentals
Hirai, Masahiro; Kanakogi, Yasuhiro – Developmental Science, 2019
The theory of natural pedagogy has proposed that infants can use ostensive signals, including eye contact, infant-directed speech, and contingency to learn from others. However, the role of bodily gestures, such as hand-waving, in social learning has been largely ignored. To address this gap in the literature, this study sought to determine…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Teaching Methods, Infants, Infant Behavior
Yow, W. Quin; Li, Xiaoqian; Lam, Sarah; Gliga, Teodora; Chong, Yap Seng; Kwek, Kenneth; Broekman, Birit F. P. – Developmental Science, 2017
Research has demonstrated a bilingual advantage in how young children use referential cues such as eye gaze and pointing gesture to locate an object or to categorize objects. This study investigated the use of referential cues (i.e. eye gaze) in fast mapping in three groups of children that differed in their language exposure. One hundred and…
Descriptors: Young Children, Bilingualism, Cues, Eye Movements
Nurmsoo, Erika; Einav, Shiri; Hood, Bruce M. – Developmental Science, 2012
This study examined children's ability to use mutual eye gaze as a cue to friendships in others. In Experiment 1, following a discussion about friendship, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds were shown animations in which three cartoon children looked at one another, and were told that one target character had a best friend. Although all age groups accurately…
Descriptors: Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Cartoons, Friendship
Kaminski, Juliane; Schulz, Linda; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Science, 2012
Domestic dogs comprehend human gestural communication in a way that other animal species do not. But little is known about the specific cues they use to determine when human communication is intended for them. In a series of four studies, we confronted both adult dogs and young dog puppies with object choice tasks in which a human indicated one of…
Descriptors: Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Infants, Human Body
Riby, Deborah M.; Doherty-Sneddon, Gwyneth; Whittle, Lisa – Developmental Science, 2012
Visual communication cues facilitate interpersonal communication. It is important that we look at faces to retrieve and subsequently process such cues. It is also important that we sometimes look away from faces as they increase cognitive load that may interfere with online processing. Indeed, when typically developing individuals hold face gaze…
Descriptors: Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Interpersonal Communication, Autism
Leekam, Susan R.; Solomon, Tracy L.; Teoh, Yee-San – Developmental Science, 2010
Three experiments investigated the effect of an adult's social cues on 2- and 3-year-old children's ability to use a sign or symbol to locate a hidden object. Results showed that an adult's positive, engaging facial expression facilitated children's ability to identify the correct referent, particularly for 3-year-olds. A neutral facial expression…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Adults
Teufel, Christoph; Gutmann, Anke; Pirow, Ralph; Fischer, Julia – Developmental Science, 2010
Gaze-following, the tendency to direct one's attention to locations looked at by others, is a crucial aspect of social cognition in human and nonhuman primates. Whereas the development of gaze-following has been intensely studied in human infants, its early ontogeny in nonhuman primates has received little attention. Combining longitudinal and…
Descriptors: Cues, Environmental Influences, Nonverbal Communication, Social Cognition
Vaidya, Chandan J.; Foss-Feig, Jennifer; Shook, Devon; Kaplan, Lauren; Kenworthy, Lauren; Gaillard, William D. – Developmental Science, 2011
Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to examine functional anatomy of attention to social (eye gaze) and nonsocial (arrow) communicative stimuli in late childhood and in a disorder defined by atypical processing of social stimuli, Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Children responded to a target word ("LEFT"/"RIGHT") in the context of a…
Descriptors: Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Autism, Brain
Birch, Susan A. J.; Akmal, Nazanin; Frampton, Kristen L. – Developmental Science, 2010
Data from three experiments provide the first evidence that children, at least as young as age two, are vigilant of others' non-verbal cues to credibility, and flexibly use these cues to facilitate learning. Experiment 1 revealed that 2- and 3-year-olds prefer to learn about objects from someone who appears, through non-verbal cues, to be…
Descriptors: Cues, Credibility, Nonverbal Communication, Toddlers
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2