NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Source
Developmental Psychology105
Audience
Researchers6
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 105 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sehl, Claudia G.; Denison, Stephanie; Friedman, Ori – Developmental Psychology, 2023
Children have a robust social preference for people similar to them, like those who share their language, accent, and race. In the present research, we show that this preference can diminish when children consider who they want to learn about. Across three experiments, 4- to 6-year-olds (total N = 160; 74 female, 86 male, from the Waterloo region…
Descriptors: Childrens Attitudes, Inferences, Social Cognition, Familiarity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Marno, Hanna – Developmental Psychology, 2021
During everyday conversations, young children are often challenged with the task of correctly identifying the referent of novel words. What is their primary aim when they try to do so? We propose that by being motivated to successfully participate in communicative interactions, children primarily aim at comprehending what the speaker intends to…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Interpersonal Communication, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kragness, Haley E.; Ullah, Farhat; Chan, Emma; Moses, Rachel; Cirelli, Laura K. – Developmental Psychology, 2022
Around the world, musical engagement frequently involves movement. Most adults easily clap or sway to a wide range of tempos, even without formal musical training. The link between movement and music emerges early--young infants move more rhythmically to music than speech, but do not reliably align their movements to the beat. Laboratory work…
Descriptors: Music Activities, Familiarity, Motion, Dance
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rioux, Camille; Wertz, Annie E. – Developmental Psychology, 2021
Infants avoid touching plants. Here we examine for the first time whether infants are also reluctant to touch plant foods. We hypothesized that infants would avoid plant foods because food neophobia--the avoidance of novel foods--is particularly strong for fruits and vegetables. However, we predicted that infants would avoid processed plant foods…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Infants, Food, Fear
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Speranza, Trinidad B.; Ramenzoni, Verónica C. – Developmental Psychology, 2022
Our ability to perceive our own and other people's bodies is critical to the success of social interactions. Research has shown that adults have a distorted perception of their own body and those of other adults. However, these studies ask perceivers to estimate for adults with a similar bodily make-up. This study explored the developmental…
Descriptors: Human Body, Self Concept, Developmental Stages, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Natalie Bleijlevens; Tanya Behne – Developmental Psychology, 2024
Upon hearing a novel label, listeners tend to assume that it refers to a novel, rather than a familiar object. While this disambiguation or mutual exclusivity (ME) effect has been robustly shown across development, it is unclear what it involves. Do listeners use their pragmatic and lexical knowledge to exclude the familiar object and thus select…
Descriptors: Ambiguity (Semantics), Toddlers, Adults, Cognitive Mapping
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Thiele, Maleen; Hepach, Robert; Michel, Christine; Haun, Daniety B. M. – Developmental Psychology, 2021
In direct interactions with others, 9-month-old infants' learning about objects is facilitated when the interaction partner addresses the infant through eye contact before looking toward an object. In this study we investigated whether similar factors promote infants' observational learning from third-party interactions. In Experiment 1,…
Descriptors: Infants, Interaction, Cognitive Processes, Eye Movements
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ponari, Marta; Norbury, Courtenay Frazier; Vigliocco, Gabriella – Developmental Psychology, 2020
A recent study by Ponari, Norbury, and Vigliocco (2018), showed that emotional valence (i.e. whether a word evokes positive, negative, or no affect) predicts age-of-acquisition ratings and that up to the age of 8-9, children know abstract emotional words better than neutral ones. On the basis of these findings, emotional valence has been argued to…
Descriptors: Emotional Response, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Language Acquisition, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Goddu, Mariel K.; Gopnik, Alison – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Novel causal systems pose a problem of variable choice: How can a reasoner decide which variable is causally relevant? Which variable in the system should a learner manipulate to try to produce a desired, yet unfamiliar, casual outcome? In much causal reasoning research, participants learn how a particular set of preselected variables produce a…
Descriptors: Young Children, Causal Models, Logical Thinking, Inferences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ruba, Ashley L.; Meltzoff, Andrew N.; Repacholi, Betty M. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Accurate perception of emotional (facial) expressions is an essential social skill. It is currently debated whether emotion categorization in infancy emerges in a "broad-to-narrow" pattern and the degree to which language influences this process. We used an habituation paradigm to explore (a) whether 14- and 18-month-old infants perceive…
Descriptors: Infants, Nonverbal Communication, Emotional Response, Toddlers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Armstrong-Carter, Emma; Garrett, Shedrick L.; Nick, Elizabeth A.; Prinstein, Mitchell J.; Telzer, Eva H. – Developmental Psychology, 2023
This longitudinal ecological momentary assessment study examined whether adolescents' use of social media to interact with peers relates to their experiences of social connectedness, social craving, and sensation seeking on an hourly level. Further, we investigated whether these associations differ for adolescents who were nominated by their peers…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Social Media, Individual Differences, Interaction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kirkorian, Heather L.; Travers, Brittany G.; Jiang, Matthew J.; Choi, Koeun; Rosengren, Karl S.; Pavalko, Porter; Tolkin, Emma – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Young children's growing access to touchscreen technology represents one of many contextual factors that may influence development. The focus of the current study was the impact of traditional versus electronic drawing materials on the quality of children's drawings during the preschool years. Young children (2-5 years, N = 73) and a comparison…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Freehand Drawing, Adults, Gender Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brink, Kimberly A.; Wellman, Henry M. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Children acquire extensive knowledge from others. Today, children receive information from not only people but also technological devices, like social robots. Two studies assessed whether young children appropriately trust technological informants. One hundred and four 3-year-olds learned the names of novel objects from either a pair of social…
Descriptors: Robotics, Trust (Psychology), Toddlers, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zupan, Zorana; Blagrove, Elisabeth; Watson, Derrick G. – Developmental Psychology, 2018
Adults can ignore old and prioritize newly arriving visual stimuli, enabling optimal goal-directed search (visual marking; Watson & Humphreys, 1997). However, the ability to use time of appearance to enhance visual search is currently absent in work on attentional development in children. Experiment 1 examined children's (6-, 8-, and…
Descriptors: Attention, Time, Children, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tang, Ping; Xu Rattanasone, Nan; Yuen, Ivan; Gao, Liqun; Demuth, Katherine – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Phonological processes result in surface variants of the same words across phonological contexts, posing potential word learning challenges for children. Mandarin tone sandhi is a tonal process changing Tone 3 (T3) in different tonal and syntactic contexts, resulting in allophonic variants of T3 in connected speech. Previous studies found that…
Descriptors: Mandarin Chinese, Phonology, Language Acquisition, Young Children
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7