NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 9 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Krajcsi, Attila; Reynvoet, Bert – Developmental Science, 2024
Initial acquisition of the first symbolic numbers is measured with the Give a Number (GaN) task. According to the classic method, it is assumed that children who know only 1, 2, 3, or 4 in the GaN task, (termed separately one-, two-, three-, and four-knowers, or collectively subset-knowers) have only a limited conceptual understanding of numbers.…
Descriptors: Numbers, Number Concepts, Symbols (Mathematics), Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Debeer, Dries; Vanbecelaere, Stefanie; Van Den Noortgate, Wim; Reynvoet, Bert; Depaepe, Fien – British Journal of Educational Technology, 2021
During the last decade, many governments and ed-tech companies have demonstrated an increased interest in digital personalised learning, which resulted in a variety of often game-like adaptive learning environments. However, there has been limited attention for the impact of these personalised learning technologies on children's learning…
Descriptors: Educational Games, Educational Technology, Individualized Instruction, Instructional Effectiveness
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vanbecelaere, Stefanie; Cornillie, Frederik; Sasanguie, Delphine; Reynvoet, Bert; Depaepe, Fien – British Journal of Educational Technology, 2021
Adaptive educational games provide new opportunities to train early numerical skills. However, empirical evidence for the effectiveness of adaptive educational games is scarce. This study investigated the effectiveness of an adaptive game compared to a nonadaptive game in terms of cognitive, noncognitive and efficiency outcomes. In total, 84…
Descriptors: Instructional Effectiveness, Educational Games, Mathematics Instruction, Numeracy
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gebuis, Titia; Reynvoet, Bert – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2012
To date, researchers investigating nonsymbolic number processes devoted little attention to the visual properties of their stimuli. This is unexpected, as nonsymbolic number is defined by its visual characteristics. When number changes, its visual properties change accordingly. In this study, we investigated the influence of different visual…
Descriptors: Number Systems, Cues, Stimuli, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sasanguie, Delphine; Gobel, Silke M.; Moll, Kristina; Smets, Karolien; Reynvoet, Bert – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
In this study, the performance of typically developing 6- to 8-year-old children on an approximate number discrimination task, a symbolic comparison task, and a symbolic and nonsymbolic number line estimation task was examined. For the first time, children's performances on these basic cognitive number processing tasks were explicitly contrasted…
Descriptors: Mathematics Achievement, Numbers, Symbols (Mathematics), Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Campbell, Jamie I. D.; Reynvoet, Bert – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
Previous research has shown that time to name single-digit Arabic numbers is about 15 ms slower when naming trials are interleaved with simple multiplication (e.g., state product of 2 x 3) than when naming digits is interleaved with magnitude comparison (e.g., state larger; 2 [arrow up] 3). To explain this phenomenon, J. I. D. Campbell and A. W.…
Descriptors: Semantics, Priming, Reaction Time, Numbers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Reynvoet, Bert; De Smedt, Bert; Van den Bussche, Eva – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
The comparison distance effect (CDE), whereby discriminating between two numbers that are far apart is easier than discriminating between two numbers that are close, has been considered as an important indicator of how people represent magnitudes internally. However, the underlying mechanism of this CDE is still unclear. We tried to shed further…
Descriptors: Numbers, Language Processing, Grade 5, Grade 1
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gevers, Wim; Verguts, Tom; Reynvoet, Bert; Caessens, Bernie; Fias, Wim – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
The SNARC (spatial numerical associations of response codes) effect reflects the tendency to respond faster with the left hand to relatively small numbers and with the right hand to relatively large numbers (S. Dehaene, S. Bossini, & P. Giraux, 1993). Using computational modeling, the present article aims to provide a framework for conceptualizing…
Descriptors: Numbers, Scientific Concepts, Task Analysis, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Van Opstal, Filip; Reynvoet, Bert; Verguts, Tom – Cognition, 2005
Recently, [Kunde, W., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2003). Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition. "Cognition," 88, 223-242] used a masked priming paradigm to argue that neither the "elaborate processing" or the "evolving automaticity" view can account for the processing of unconscious numerical stimuli. In our Experiment 1 we…
Descriptors: Numbers, Cognitive Processes, Experiments, Cues