Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 16 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Asmuth, Jennifer | 1 |
Atkinson, T. P. | 1 |
Baroody, Arthur J. | 1 |
Black, Miriam T. | 1 |
Bloomfield, Amber | 1 |
Clements, Douglas H. | 1 |
De Smedt, Bert | 1 |
Demana, Franklin | 1 |
Devlin, Keith | 1 |
Fein, Linda Abby | 1 |
Fischer, Martin H. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Audience
Practitioners | 7 |
Teachers | 4 |
Parents | 1 |
Researchers | 1 |
Location
China | 2 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
De Smedt, Bert – Research in Mathematics Education, 2019
In this commentary, I reflect from a neurocognitive perspective on the four chapters on natural number development included in this section. These chapters show that the development of seemingly basic number processing is much more complex than is often portrayed in neurocognitive research. The chapters collectively illustrate that children's…
Descriptors: Numbers, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level, Cognitive Development
Stacey, Kaye – Research in Mathematics Education, 2018
These four chapters describe studies of using models for integer addition and subtraction. The models draw principally on the two grounding metaphors of object collection and motion along a path. A strength of all chapters is detailed analysis of how the models are and can be implemented and how they influence student's learning. Together the…
Descriptors: Mathematics Instruction, Mathematical Models, Numbers, Addition
Baroody, Arthur J. – Research in Mathematics Education, 2018
Ginsburg (1977) observed that children typically develop surprisingly powerful informal (everyday) knowledge of mathematics and that mathematical learning difficulties often arise when formal instruction does not build on this existing knowledge. By using meaningful analogies teachers can help connect new formal instruction to students' existing…
Descriptors: Mathematics Instruction, Teaching Methods, Logical Thinking, Numbers
Peled, Irit; Klemer, Anat – Research in Mathematics Education, 2018
The crucial role of teachers in introducing integers to children is highlighted in chapters 8-10, comprising this section. The three chapters discuss (prospective) teachers' conceptions of integer equations, of children's thinking about integer expressions, and of the role of some didactical models used in teaching integer addition and…
Descriptors: Mathematics Teachers, Mathematics Instruction, Pedagogical Content Knowledge, Numbers
Garner, Mary – Measurement: Interdisciplinary Research and Perspectives, 2013
In "How Is Testing Supposed to Improve Schooling," Haertel describes seven broad mechanisms whereby testing is used to improve schooling (this issue). The first four are direct mechanisms, meaning that "test scores are taken as indicators of some underlying construct and on that basis scores are used to guide some decision or draw some…
Descriptors: Testing, Early Intervention, Educational Improvement, Change Strategies
Fischer, Martin H.; Shaki, Samuel – Brain and Cognition, 2011
The commentary by Treccani and Umilta (2011) on our recent paper in this journal (Fischer et al., 2010) usefully broadens the perspective on numerical cognition for a general readership and clarifies (or raises) some fundamental issues of cognitive representation. Our response to this commentary is organized into three main sections that focus…
Descriptors: Numbers, Cognitive Processes, Spatial Ability, Association (Psychology)
Roth, Wolff-Michael – Mathematics Education Research Journal, 2012
Cultural-historical activity theory--with historical roots in dialectical materialism and the social psychology to which it has given rise--has experienced exponential growth in its acceptance by scholars interested in understanding knowing and learning writ large. In education, this theory has constituted something like a well kept secret that is…
Descriptors: Mathematics Education, Social Systems, Social Psychology, Mathematics
Wilson, W. Stephen – AASA Journal of Scholarship & Practice, 2009
This article first describes some of the basic skills and knowledge that a solid elementary school mathematics foundation requires. It then elaborates on several points germane to these practices. These are then followed with a discussion and conclude with final comments and suggestions for future research. The article sets out the five…
Descriptors: Elementary School Mathematics, Needs Assessment, Mathematics Skills, Knowledge Level
Margolis, Eric; Laurence, Stephen – Cognition, 2008
Theories of number concepts often suppose that the natural numbers are acquired as children learn to count and as they draw an induction based on their interpretation of the first few count words. In a bold critique of this general approach, Rips, Asmuth, Bloomfield [Rips, L., Asmuth, J. & Bloomfield, A. (2006). Giving the boot to the bootstrap:…
Descriptors: Numbers, Learning Strategies, Number Concepts, Inferences
Maruszewski, Richard – College Mathematics Journal, 2009
In 1225 Fibonacci visited the court of the Holy Roman Emperor, Frederick II. Because Frederick was an important patron of learning, this visit was important to Fibonacci. During the audience, Frederick's court mathematician posed three problems to test Fibonacci. The third was to find the real solution to the equation: x[superscript 3] +…
Descriptors: Computation, Mathematics Instruction, College Mathematics, Problem Solving
Kadosh, Roi Cohen; Tzelgov, Joseph; Henik, Avishai – Cognition, 2008
Are small and large numbers represented similarly or differently on the mental number line? The size effect was used to argue that numbers are represented differently. However, recently it has been argued that the size effect is due to the comparison task and is not derived from the mental number line per se. Namely, it is due to the way that the…
Descriptors: Measurement Techniques, Numbers, Computation, Effect Size
Glaister, P. – International Journal of Mathematical Education in Science and Technology, 2008
A generalization of a well-known result for the arctangent function poses a number of interesting questions concerning the existence of integer solutions of related problems.
Descriptors: Problem Solving, Mathematics Instruction, Trigonometry, Generalization
Wolfe, Joanna – College Composition and Communication, 2010
Contemporary argument increasingly relies on quantitative information and reasoning, yet our profession neglects to view these means of persuasion as central to rhetorical arts. Such omission ironically serves to privilege quantitative arguments as above "mere rhetoric." Changes are needed to our textbooks, writing assignments, and instructor…
Descriptors: Writing Assignments, Rhetoric, Student Attitudes, Textbooks
Rips, Lance J.; Asmuth, Jennifer; Bloomfield, Amber – Cognition, 2006
According to one theory about how children learn the concept of natural numbers, they first determine that "one", "two", and "three" denote the size of sets containing the relevant number of items. They then make the following inductive inference (the Bootstrap): The next number word in the counting series denotes the size of the sets you get by…
Descriptors: Numbers, Number Concepts, Inferences, Computation
It Pays to Compare! Using Comparison to Help Build Students' Flexibility in Mathematics. Newsletter.
Star, Jon R. – Center for Comprehensive School Reform and Improvement, 2008
Flexible knowledge, knowing multiple approaches for solving problems, is a hallmark of expertise in mathematics. Frequently, the author writes, students memorize only one method of solving a certain kind of problem, without understanding what they are doing, why a given strategy works, and whether there are alternative solution methods. Comparison…
Descriptors: Numbers, Mathematics Instruction, Problem Solving, Mathematics Skills