Publication Date
In 2025 | 24 |
Since 2024 | 93 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 326 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 910 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1950 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Teachers | 234 |
Practitioners | 173 |
Students | 41 |
Researchers | 25 |
Parents | 9 |
Administrators | 2 |
Media Staff | 2 |
Policymakers | 2 |
Location
Australia | 92 |
Turkey | 43 |
Canada | 31 |
Germany | 27 |
Indonesia | 23 |
China | 22 |
United Kingdom | 22 |
United States | 21 |
Taiwan | 19 |
Greece | 18 |
South Africa | 18 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Bilingual Education Act 1968 | 3 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 3 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 2 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 2 |
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 5 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 7 |
Does not meet standards | 2 |
Ahu Canogullari; Mine Isiksal-Bostan – European Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 2024
The current research aimed to unpack teachers' knowledge of integers by investigating how they used the number line and counter models to represent the two meanings of division (i.e., partitive and measurement). The participants were three middle school mathematics teachers working in different cities in Türkiye. Data consisted of teachers'…
Descriptors: Middle School Teachers, Mathematics Teachers, Knowledge Level, Numbers
Sevim Nuraydin; Johannes Stricker; Michael Schneider – Child Development, 2024
The number line estimation task is frequently used to measure children's numerical magnitude understanding. It is unclear whether the resulting straight, horizontal, left-to-right-oriented estimate patterns indicate task constraints or children's intuitive number--space mapping. Three- to six-year-old children (N = 72, M[subscript age] = 4.89, 56%…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Numbers, Mathematics Skills, Numeracy
Andrews-Larson, Christine; Wilson, Jonee; Mauntel, Matthew; Smith, Jessica; Hawthorne-Glover, Thomeca – Mathematics Teacher: Learning and Teaching PK-12, 2023
Integer addition and subtraction entail significant conceptual shifts for many students, and teachers often offer rules intended to help mitigate this challenge. When these rules are not connected to sense making, a misremembered rule can function as a barrier to student learning. In this article, the authors highlight the work of an expert…
Descriptors: Numbers, Subtraction, Thinking Skills, Mathematics Education
Marlin E. Blaine – Hispania, 2023
This article describes "Verse Chronograms" in the Cabrera Portrait (1750) of Sor Juana. The author explains the meaning of three hard-to-see sets of verses inscribed on the canvas, that are a kind of text known as a chronogram, a literary puzzle in which the numerical values of letters that correspond to Roman numerals are added up so…
Descriptors: Painting (Visual Arts), Portraiture, Puzzles, Alphabets
Sokolowski, H. Moriah; Merkley, Rebecca; Kingissepp, Sarah Samantha Bray; Vaikuntharajan, Praja; Ansari, Daniel – Developmental Science, 2022
Which dimension of a set of objects is more salient to young children: number or size? The "Build-A-Train" task was developed and used to examine whether children spontaneously use a number or physical size approach on an un-cued matching task. In the Build-A-Train task, an experimenter assembles a train using one to five blocks of a…
Descriptors: Young Children, Task Analysis, Numbers, Knowledge Level
Dolscheid, Sarah; Ostrowski, Lea; Verlage, Heiko – International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 2022
The approximate number system (ANS) is foundational to numerical cognition. Whereas some studies hint at intact ANS skills in individuals with Down syndrome (DS), others report impaired ANS abilities. To reconcile these discrepant findings, we investigated ANS skills of children with DS and typically developing (TD) children in a non-symbolic…
Descriptors: Numeracy, Numbers, Down Syndrome, Mathematics Skills
Morris, Steven L. – Physics Teacher, 2022
It is difficult for an instructor to just make up valid numbers for B[subscript x], B[subscript y], B[subscript z], E[subscript x], E[subscript y], and E[subscript z] in the creation of homework problems and test questions calculating the Poynting vector. In this paper, 25 examples are given of the electric and magnetic fields of electromagnetic…
Descriptors: Science Instruction, Physics, Energy, Magnets
Dolgunsöz, Emrah – Reading Matrix: An International Online Journal, 2021
Numbers (i.e. 45) are symbols that are common in reading texts in various forms such as dates, percentages, and quantity expressions. Each digit corresponds to a word, unlike words in which each letter represents a phoneme. Despite their high frequency in reading texts, eye movement research on numeral processing is still rare. Numerals are easy…
Descriptors: Numbers, Number Concepts, Second Language Learning, Oral Reading
Ling Zhang; Naiqing Song; Guowei Wu; Jinfa Cai – Educational Studies in Mathematics, 2025
This study concerns the cognitive process of mathematical problem posing, conceptualized in three stages: understanding the task, constructing the problem, and expressing the problem. We used the eye tracker and think-aloud methods to deeply explore students' behavior in these three stages of problem posing, especially focusing on investigating…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Mathematics Skills, Problem Solving, Eye Movements
Margaret M. Flores; Vanessa M. Hinton; Kelly B. Schweck – Remedial and Special Education, 2024
This study's purpose was to examine the effects of the concrete-representational-abstract integrated (CRA-I) sequence on the performance of students who struggled with rational number concepts. Three students in southeastern U.S. fifth grade class participated in the study. The CRA-I intervention was grounded in the principles of explicit…
Descriptors: Grade 5, Elementary School Mathematics, Rural Schools, Numbers
Walter Gander – Informatics in Education, 2024
When the new programming language Pascal was developed in the 1970's, Walter Gander did not like it because because many features which he appreciated in prior programming languages were missing in Pascal. For example the block structure was gone, there were no dynamical arrays, no functions or procedures were allowed as parameters of a procedure,…
Descriptors: Computer Software, Programming Languages, Algorithms, Automation
Ririn Dwi Agustin; Cholis Sa'dijah; Susiswo; Sukoriyanto – Pegem Journal of Education and Instruction, 2024
This study aimed to describe students' semantic representation obstacles in solving geometric problems. There are three types of obstacles, that are ontogeny, epistemology, and didactics obstacles. This study was carried out on three subjects with different obstacles and focused on math semantic representations in solving geometry problems. The…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Preservice Teacher Education, Preservice Teachers, Geometry
Dotan, Dror – Cognitive Science, 2023
Reading numbers aloud involves visual processes that analyze the digit string and verbal processes that produce the number words. Cognitive models of number reading assume that information flows from the visual input to the verbal production processes--a feed-forward processing mode in which the verbal production depends on the visual input but…
Descriptors: Reading Processes, Hebrew, Arabic, Visual Stimuli
Powell, Sarah R.; Nelson, Gena – Psychology in the Schools, 2021
To understand misconceptions with rational numbers (i.e., fractions, decimals, and percentages), we administered an assessment of rational numbers to 331 undergraduate students from a 4-year university. The assessment included 41 items categorized as measuring foundational understanding, calculations, or word problems. We coded each student's…
Descriptors: Undergraduate Students, Misconceptions, Number Concepts, Numbers
Wilkey, Eric D.; Shanley, Lina; Sabb, Fred; Ansari, Daniel; Cohen, Jason C.; Men, Virany; Heller, Nicole A.; Clarke, Ben – Grantee Submission, 2021
Children's ability to discriminate nonsymbolic number (e.g. the number of items in a set) is a commonly studied predictor of later math skills. Number discrimination improves throughout development, but what drives this improvement is unclear. Competing theories suggest it may be due to a sharpening numerical representation or an improved ability…
Descriptors: Numbers, Mathematics Skills, Predictor Variables, Number Concepts